The sudden outbreak and rapid rate of transmission of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) during its initial onset followed by a later surge in cases and hospitalization among children caused by the Delta SARS-CoV-2 variant in the U.S. [Siegel et al., 2021 ] created a critical need for effective health communication about coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) for children. Early studies indicated that children infected with the virus often were asymptomatic [Miri et al., 2020 ], experienced milder symptoms than adults [Brodin, 2020 ; Ludvigsson, 2020 ; Oterino Serrano et al., 2020 ], and overall, the number of cases among children were less frequent than those among adults [Buonsenso et al., 2020 ; Zhu et al., 2021 ]. Subsequently, early concerns focused primarily on children’s role in transmission of the virus to others. However, greater concern emerged with the marked rise in cases of the virus among children and adolescents from June to August 2021 that led to more visits to hospital emergency departments and an increase in hospital admissions, especially in states with lower vaccination rates [Siegel et al., 2021 ]. During the early months of the appearance of the Delta variant, the number of COVID-19 cases among children and adolescents increased by almost five-fold in 14 different states in the U.S. [Delahoy et al., 2021 ]. Both early and later concerns about COVID-19 infection among children underscored a critical need for effective health messaging about COVID-19 in order to promote health protection behaviors and good hygiene practices among children.
In May 2020, a few months after official news reports of the outbreak of the pandemic in the U.S., the CDC posted recommendations for communicating about COVID-19 to children on their website [Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020 ]. Communicating about science and health with children is especially difficult because of the complexity of this information. Science and health communication about COVID-19 to children presented an unusually unique challenge. In addition to the difficulties in communicating complex information to a young audience, typical communication channels were more scattered during the pandemic because of the sudden shift from in-person, classroom instruction to online, remote instruction [García and Weiss, 2020 ]. As schools closed, families sheltered-in-place, and children turned to online learning during the early months of the pandemic [Cluver et al., 2020 ; UNESCO, 2020 ], increased online activity led to unprecedented access to digital media for children [UNICEF, 2020 ].
Even prior to the COVID-19 outbreak, children’s and adolescents’ online media use and online presence had escalated from levels reported in previous years [Anderson and Jiang, 2018 ; Gelmez Burakgazi and Yildirim, 2014 ]. In recent years, YouTube has emerged as one of the most popular online platforms for children [Kang, Ha and Velasco, 2017 ; Neumann and Herodotou, 2020a ; Neumann and Herodotou, 2020b ; van Kessel, 2019 ]. During the pandemic, YouTube videos became readily available sources of scientific, health, and medical information about COVID-19 for children. The emergence of children’s videos about COVID-19 provided an opportunity to investigate how scientific, health, and medical information about the virus was communicated to children through this popular digital media platform during a global pandemic. However, few prior studies have examined messaging approaches and design features found in children’s YouTube videos or their potential effects on children [Jones and Cuthrell, 2011 ; Neumann and Herodotou, 2020a ].
The present study addressed this research gap by taking an important first step in examining the content of children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 posted during the first eight months of the pandemic in the U.S. This study contributed to extant research in science and health communication in several important ways. First, drawing on social cognitive theory, this study examined the attributes of primary characters and the extent to which primary characters demonstrated and modeled recommended health protection behaviors. Second, this study investigated the presence and diverse representation of expert characters (doctors, nurses, public health officials, psychiatrists, scientists) in these videos. Third, this study examined the discourse frames used to communicate scientific and health information in these videos. Fourth, this study assessed changes in the use of discourse frames in children’s videos posted before and after issuance of CDC guidelines for health communication about COVID-19 for children. The YouTube children’s videos analyzed for this study featured content that targeted children from preschool-age (4 and under) to early adolescence (9–12) as specified in YouTube content settings designations for children [YouTube, 2021 ].
1 Literature review
1.1 Observational learning from media role models
Social cognitive theory describes, predicts, and explains how children learn to imitate behavior modeled by others through “observational learning” [Bandura, 2001 , p. 270] and “identificatory learning” [Bandura, 1963 , p. 591; Bandura, 1969 ]. Research on social cognitive theory has highlighted the importance of media role models in influencing children’s attitudes and behaviors [Bandura, 2002 ; Bandura, 2009 ; Bandura, Ross and Ross, 1963a ; Bandura, Ross and Ross, 1963b ]. According to this theory, “media both teach new forms of behavior and create motivators for action by altering people’s value preferences, efficacy beliefs, outcome expectations, and perception of opportunity structures” [Bandura, 2001 , p. 286]. Media characters, as vicarious role models, are important because they can influence children’s attitudes and behaviors through both the information they convey and the behaviors they model [Bandura, 2001 ; Bandura, Ross and Ross, 1963b ].
Learning from media role models may be even more pronounced when children identify with characters in the media content [Hoffner and Buchanan, 2005 ; Moyer-Gusé, 2008 ; Steinke et al., 2012 ]. Research specifically focused on observational learning and social modelling from health messages has found that observation of others demonstrating a desired behavior can indeed promote adoption of healthy behaviors. One study found that “[s]eeing other people engaged in a certain practice (e.g., eating fruit and vegetables) suggests that the practice is desirable, safe, and beneficial to one’s well-being” [Samson, Nanne and Buijzen, 2021 , p. 3]. Research also has found that same-gender, same-race, and same-ethnicity role models are especially important for promoting positive behavior among children and adolescents [Carsten Conner and Danielson, 2016 ; Villiers et al., 2015 ].
1.2 YouTube, children, and science communication
YouTube has become an easily accessible and popular online source of information for children [Jaakkola, 2020 ; Neumann and Herodotou, 2020b ; Smith, Toor and van Kessel, 2018 ] and the preferred digital media platform for many young children [Marsh et al., 2015 ]. According to a recent report by the Pew Research Center, 81 percent of parents with children under 11 reported that they allowed their child to watch YouTube content, and 34 percent of parents indicated that their child watched YouTube content regularly [Smith, Toor and van Kessel, 2018 ]. Children as young as 12 to 17 months of age view YouTube videos [Hourcade et al., 2015 ]. Many of the multimedia content features of YouTube videos (e.g. audio, text, special effects, animated characters) have been found to attract children’s attention [Neumann and Herodotou, 2020b ]. However, research on children’s learning from YouTube video content is limited. One study found that children ages 6 to 24 months imitated dance moves on YouTube videos, identified with cartoon and fairy-tale characters in the videos, and reported enjoying advertisements for familiar foods [Yadav et al., 2018 ]. However, this study found while children identified with and imitated the behavior of YouTube characters, they did not learn much else from the videos [Yadav et al., 2018 ].
Science and health communication through digital media platforms, like YouTube, has become more common for communicating scientific and health information to non-expert audiences [Anderson, Brossard and Scheufele, 2010 ; Brossard and Scheufele, 2013 ; Hunter, 2020 ; Papadopoulos, 2018 ; Renee, 2018 ; Simis-Wilkinson et al., 2018 ]. Prior science communication research on YouTube has focused primarily on its use in enhancing science classroom instruction. One study found that YouTube music videos about the big bang theory, theory of evolution, the periodic table of the elements, and the human brain were effective in supporting science outreach efforts to promote positive images of science and foster science engagement for students as well as for introducing scientific ideas and concepts in educational settings [Allgaier, 2013 ]. YouTube videos also have been found effective for enhancing engagement and learning in undergraduate chemistry classes [Smith, 2014 ], and learning about science history and promoting participation in virtual field trips [Everhart, 2009 ]. These studies highlight the potential for effective science and health communication to children through YouTube videos.
1.3 Science communication for children
Little prior research has explored best practices for communicating scientific, health, and medical information to children through digital media. Prior research in this area has focused primarily on learning from viewing science television programs or behavioral changes from viewing public service announcements (PSAs) or television commercials promoting healthy behaviors. Specifically, prior science communication research has identified design features of television programs most likely to promote enjoyment and learning for children [Mares, Cantor and Steinbach, 1999 ]. Other research has established that specific genre and character attributes best promote identification with scientist characters on television programs watched by adolescents [Steinke et al., 2012 ]. Research also has found that anthropomorphic depictions — those featuring human characteristics to depict science concepts — promote children’s learning from science television shows [Bonus and Mares, 2018 ].
Other studies have focused specifically on message design strategies that promote science understanding in formal and informal science educational environments. For example, one study found that elementary children’s science learning from an educational science television program was enhanced when scientific content was presented “in the format of a pop-out from a feature story” and highlighted the scientific aspects of everyday events [Mares, Cantor and Steinbach, 1999 ]. Another study found that one half of children’s television programs used refutation to teach science concepts and one-third of these programs conveyed science concepts using anthropomorphism, or giving animal characters human-like features and traits [Bonus and Mares, 2018 , p. 451]. This study found that the use of refutation had no effect on pre-school age children’s science learning; however, the use of anthropomorphism led to a positive increase in children’s science knowledge. Although not specifically focused on science learning from media, another study noted that spacing out learning events over time rather than in rapid succession can facilitate the acquisition of science concepts for early elementary school children [Vlach and Sandhofer, 2012 ].
Prior health communication research has examined the use of message design and different types of appeals [Nicolini, Cassia and Bellotto, 2017 ] in public health campaigns and interventions to best promote healthy behavior for a variety of health issues such as nutrition [Nicolini, Cassia and Bellotto, 2017 ; Tan et al., 2018 ], teen smoking [Farrelly, Niederdeppe and Yarsevich, 2003 ; Rhodes, Ralston and Bigsby, 2016 ; Zhao and Cai, 2016 ], and alcohol consumption [Adnsager, Austin and Pinkleton, 2001 ; Pinkleton, Austin and Fujioka, 2001 ; Pinkleton, Austin and Van de Vord, 2010 ; Slater et al., 1996 ]. Research has examined the use of different types of appeals in health communication messages for children. For example, a study of 8- to 12-year-old children found that the use of threat appeals in audio-visual advertisements about unhealthy eating habits was more effective than usual appeals in promoting healthy eating behaviors, particularly if children were left alone to deal with post-exposure affective reactions [Charry and Demoulin, 2012 ]. This study also found that threat appeals most often elicited the emotions of sadness, worry, and disgust, rather than fear, following exposure [Charry and Demoulin, 2012 ]. By contrast, a study of 7- to 18-year-olds found that the use of social appeals, or images featuring large, gregarious groups of children and adolescents enjoying healthy foods, were more memorable, particularly for teenagers compared to children and preteens [Samson, Nanne and Buijzen, 2021 ]. Another study of adolescents reported that humor, fear, and nurturing appeals in PSAs were related to the intention to cut back on sugar-sweetened beverages with perceived argument strength of the health PSAs mediating direct effects of fear appeals and indirect effects of humor and nurturance appeals on intention to cut back on sugar-sweetened beverages [Bleakley et al., 2015 ].
One of the few studies focused on medical communication for children assessed the readability and use of visuals in medical research informational consent forms in the Netherlands [Grootens-Wiegers et al., 2015 ]. This study found low readability scores for consent forms and noted only three of the 22 forms used visuals [Grootens-Wiegers et al., 2015 ]. The researchers also noted that children preferred forms with visuals, especially realistic images and funny cartoons, as well as forms that included informative, clear text or captions [Grootens-Wiegers et al., 2015 ]. The researchers noted, “there is little research on the quality of medical information for children and adolescents, and there are no evidence-based insights in how health communication can be optimally adapted for this target group” [Grootens-Wiegers et al., 2015 , p. 91].
1.4 Discourse frames
Few studies have considered the use of discourse frames in science and health communication for children. Discourse is the language by which information is communicated to others. Foucault [ 1972 ] described “manifest discourse” [p. 25] simply as information that was “already said”, [p. 25] noting that “[d]iscourse must not be referred to the distant presence of the origin, but treated as and when it occurs” [p. 25]. Media discourse, described most often in the context of news discourse, has been described as “a set of interpretative packages that give meaning to an issue” [Gamson and Modigliani, 1989 , p. 3]. Research has identified framing devices in news media discourse (metaphors, exemplars, catchphrases, depictions, and visual images) that serve as communicative strategies [Gamson and Modigliani, 1989 ].
In communication research, frames have been used to describe selection of language by a source when encoding meaning in to media content. Frames have been described as “a central organizing idea for making sense of relevant events and suggesting what is at issue” [Gamson, 1989 ]. Framing highlights salience and importance, and “[t]o frame is to select some aspects of a perceived reality and make them more salient in a communicating text, in such a way as to promote a particular problem definition, causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recommendation for the item described” [Entman, 1993 , p. 52]. Communication researchers have distinguished between — equivalence framing, “a form of framing that involves manipulating the presentation of logically equivalent information”, [Cacciatore, Scheufele and Iyengar, 2016 , p. 8] — and emphasis framing, “a form of framing that involves manipulating the content of a communication” [Cacciatore, Scheufele and Iyengar, 2016 , p. 8]. Equivalency framing focuses on how information is communicated using language to produce an effect; while emphasis framing focuses on what information is communicated by different topics to produce an effect [Cacciatore, Scheufele and Iyengar, 2016 ].
Prior research on framing of science and health information has more often focused on emphasis framing, identifying and documenting specific topics [Iyengar, 1989 ; Iyengar, 1991 ] used to describe scientific issues or controversies [Detenber et al., 2018 ; Huttunen and Hildén, 2014 ; Jones-Jang et al., 2020 ; McCauley, Minsky and Viswanath, 2013 ; Stewart, 2013 ; Xie, 2015 ], rather than equivalency framing. However, meaning is shaped not only by what information is highlighted in media messages but also how information is presented in media messages.
1.5 Media framing of COVID-19
The framing of discourse about COVID-19 in the mass media during the pandemic may be instrumental in shaping public opinion and behaviors. For instance, an analysis of 1.9 million Tweets posted about COVID-19 from January 23 to March 8, 2020 identified that the most prevalent topics were updates about the number of COVID-19 cases and related deaths, but discussions of COVID-19 symptoms and treatment were notably missing [Xue et al., 2020 ]. Researchers noted that when the CDC issued its first warning about COVID-19 on Twitter on February 10, 2020, this would have been an important time to provide guidance to the public about taking preventive measures [Xue et al., 2020 ].
Visual representations of COVID-19 also contribute to framing of COVID-19, which can influence public opinion and behavior. A study of 497 coronavirus cartoons published in South African print and online newspapers during the first five months of the pandemic found that cartoons often featured “spikey blobs with evil grins”, [Joubert and Wasserman, 2020 , p. 1]. These images emphasized spikes and striking colors (green which is associated with sickness or slime or red which is associated with danger) as well as anthropomorphic features to evoke the emotion of fear. The authors argued that these visual media representations of the coronavirus were “metaphorical expressions of broader anxieties, fears and (mis)conceptions” [Joubert and Wasserman, 2020 , p. 20]. Visualizations of the coronavirus such as these contributed to the spread of disinformation during the “COVID-19 ‘infodemic”’ [Joubert and Wasserman, 2020 , p. 20] in both print [Joubert and Wasserman, 2020 ] and social media [Cinelli et al., 2020 ].
2 Research questions
This exploratory study focused on an analysis of primary characters featured in children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 and messaging approaches used to convey scientific, health, and medical information about COVID-19 during the first eight months of the pandemic in the U.S. Drawing on social cognitive theory [Bandura, 1986 ; Bandura, 2009 ] and the literature reviewed above, this study used a content analysis to (1) document primary character attributes and health prevention behavior presented in the videos, (2) examine diverse representation of expert characters, and (3) investigate the discourse frames used to communicate scientific, health, and medical information in these videos. The following research questions were posed for this study:
-
What were the demographic attributes of the primary characters featured in
children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19?
- What character types were used to portray primary characters?
- Did the primary characters demonstrate health protection behaviors?
-
Did children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 feature expert characters (medical
and health professionals and scientists)?
- What was the gender of first-appearing expert characters in the videos?
- What was the race/ethnicity of first-appearing expert characters in the videos?
- How were coronavirus characters portrayed in children’s YouTube videos?
- What discourse frames were used to convey scientific and health information about COVID-19 in children’s YouTube videos?
- Are there differences in the use of discourse frames for children’s YouTube videos created before the CDC issued guidelines for communicating about COVID-19 to children and videos created after the CDC guidelines were released?
3 Method
3.1 Sample
Google keyword searches were used to identify YouTube videos about COVID-19 that appeared online from February to September 2020. For this study, children’s YouTube videos were defined as those that specifically addressed children and youth from preschool-age to adolescence. In order to identify YouTube videos that targeted these age groups, the keywords “children/kids/teens” were used in searches for YouTube videos and two researchers watched all videos, eliminating those that addressed adult viewers or focused on news stories for adult audiences. Two searches were conducted for this study. The first search included videos posted following initial news reports of the pandemic in the U.S. The following keywords and phrases were used: “explaining COVID-19 to children/kids/teens”, “explaining social distancing to children/kids/teens”, and “explaining face masks to children/kids/teens” yielding 66 YouTube videos. The second search was conducted using key phrases derived from the CDC’s recommendations for best practices for communicating about COVID-19 with children [Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020 ]. The following key phrases were used: “explaining to children how COVID-19 spreads”, “explaining to children hand washing”, “explaining to children how to avoid close contact”, “explaining to children how to cover your nose and mouth with a mask”, “explaining to children how to cover coughs and sneezes”, “explaining to children how to clean and disinfect”, and “explaining to children how to monitor your health daily health”, yielding 90 videos. To qualify as a children’s video, the video needed to target or address preschool-aged children (4 and younger) to early adolescents (9–12) [YouTube, 2021 ]. Videos that targeted adult audiences, were longer than 20 minutes, and were duplicates were removed, yielding a total of 136 videos for analysis (see appendix A ). Most of the videos were produced and posted by non-profit and health organizations; few were produced and posted by everyday citizens (see appendix A ). While the searches did not limit the sample to educational videos, most of the videos were educational in nature. Information about each video was recorded to describe: (1) title/URL, (2) posting date, (3) posting source, (4) video play time, (5) number of views, (6) number of likes, and (7) number of dislikes (appendix A ).
3.2 Coding and procedures
Each video was treated as the unit of analysis for this content analysis. A codebook was created to guide analysis, listing examples of video content illustrating each code or variable of interest. The definitions of the variables coded for this study are listed below.
Production features , indicative of the technical characteristics of a video, were coded. Audio format was coded for voice-overs (adult vs. child), music, and other. Video format was coded for the categories of live action (cartoon vs. human), text-only, graphics-only, and other.
Demographic attributes were coded for primary characters by character type (human, animated), gender (male, female, undetermined), age (baby, child/teen, adult, elderly adult, undetermined), and race/ethnicity (White, Black, Hispanic/Latinx, Asian, undetermined). Primary characters were identified as the first appearing character or featured narrator in a video.
Character type was coded for the primary characters (doctor, scientist, other adult, child, muppet, coronavirus, or undetermined). Physical characteristics, attire, setting, and character titles mentioned in the video or appearing on screen were used to determine character type.
Expert characters included health professionals (doctors, nurses, public health officials, psychologists) and scientists. The setting in which the characters appeared on the video (hospital, doctor’s office, laboratory), the attire worn by the character (stethoscope, lab coat), and titles of characters mentioned in the video or appearing on screen were used to identify expert characters.
Coronavirus character expression was coded for visual representations of the coronavirus, which appeared in some videos. To investigate how coronavirus characters were portrayed to children, the expression of the character (“happy/smiling”, “unhappy/frowning”, “scary/frightening”, “neutral/subdued”, or other) was coded.
Discourse frames were coded for statements presented in the videos by the narrator and any other character who appeared in the video to communicate information about COVID-19. Discourse frames were identified by viewing videos and reviewing video transcripts to identify and record emergent patterns related to language use or discourse frame use in the videos. Information about COVID-19 was primarily conveyed or communicated in these videos using statements that either provided objective, factual information about COVID-19, attempted to influence or persuade viewers to adopt specific behaviors, or featured short, firm, direct commands about specific behaviors that should be adopted. Thus, the primary or dominant recurring discourse frames found were: informative , persuasive , and directive . The total number of each of these three discourse frames was identified, coded, and counted for each video. If a video presented more than one type of discourse frame, then all discourse frames used in the video were counted. The total number of discourse frames across all videos was then determined.
The informative discourse frame described facts associated with the virus. These facts were coded for statements about: 1) origin of the virus (country, animal host, animal/human host, animal consumption, and other); 2) physical appearance of the virus (shape, size, color, and other); 3) methods of transmission (animal to human, human to human, droplets, utensil sharing, surface contact, and other); 4) symptoms (fever, running nose, cough, breathing problem, vomiting, diarrhea, pneumonia, lung fluid, and other); 5) social consequences (virus sickens people, missing grandparents/friends/school, indoor/outdoor out-of-home activities, inconveniences; and other); and 6) treatment (hospitalization, quarantine, and other treatments).
The persuasive discourse frame described four different behavioral dimensions associated with the pandemic experience. These dimensions were coded for statements: 1) demonstrating empowerment (fighting the virus, being a superhero, and embodying other types of power); 2) showing fear (the virus could hurt and hospitalize you and others, virus is scary and dangerous, and other); 3) taking responsibility (doing the right thing, exhibiting cooperative behavior, taking care of others, keeping others safe, and other); 4) receiving reassurance (doctors/scientists being helpful, people staying calm, people getting better, kids being less sick than adults, virus going away, immune system overcoming virus, and other).
The directive discourse frame described or illustrated the practice of protective measures associated with slowing transmission of the virus. These statements included information about: 1) good hygiene (washing hands, using sanitizer, coughing/sneezing in tissue, coughing into elbow, not touching face, not touching others, and other); 2) self-care (getting enough rest, eating healthy, engaging in exercise, getting sunlight, and other); 3) physical distancing (staying home when sick, keeping physical distance, avoiding crowds, keeping away from grandparents, and other); 4) protective behavior (wearing a facial mask, wearing gloves, telling others when feeling sick, seeking medical care, keeping home/surfaces clean, and other); and 5) adaptive coping (talking to others when feeling overwhelmed, limiting social media/news exposure, staying connected with friends/family, and other).
3.2.1 Intercoder reliability
Two graduate assistants were trained in use of the codebook. The coders practiced using the codebook using a think-aloud procedure to discuss their coding selections, questions, and discrepancies while coding two randomly selected videos from both samples. Next, an iterative process was used to assess intercoder reliability. First, coders independently coded all variables for 10 percent of the total sample and intercoder reliability scores were calculated. Next, coders independently coded 15 percent of total sample and intercoder reliability scores were calculated. 1 At both times, coders discussed discrepancies with the research team. Discrepancies were resolved and modifications were made to the codebook. Scott’s pi [Scott, 1955 ] was used to assess the level of interrater reliability for each variable based on the procedure recommended by DFreelon.org [Freelon, 2010 ]. A total of 26 out of 29 variables achieved intercoder reliabilities ranging from .80 (one variable) to 1 (10 variables), with 15 variables with scores in the range of .83–.96. Only three variables fell below .80: two at .79 and one at .71. Intercoder reliability results were deemed largely sufficient for meeting reported standards [Lombard, Snyder-Duch and Bracken, 2002 ], considering the large number of variables and the latent nature of the video content. Following the second round of reliability checks, the two coders coded the rest of the sample independently.
4 Results
4.1 Description of videos
The overall frequency of occurrence of the following video formats in the COVID-19 children’s videos were: 108 (34.7%) for print/text only, 94 (30.2%) for graphics only, 56 (18%) for live-action animation/cartoons, and 5 (14.5%) for live-action human actors. The overall frequency of occurrence of the following audio formats was: 109 (61.6%) for music, 46 (26%) for adult voiceovers, and 18 (10.2%) for child voiceovers. Many of the videos included more than one video format or audio format; subsequently, each type of video format or audio format was coded for each video.
4.2 Primary characters
RQ1 focused on examining the demographic attributes of primary characters. Findings indicated that a total of 57 primary characters appeared in the videos. They were about equally as likely to be men or boys ( , 43.9%) or women or girls ( , 42.1%). A majority of the primary characters were white ( , 59.6%) and equally likely to be adults or elderly adults ( , 38.6%) or children/teens ( , 38.6%) (Table 1 ).
RQ1a investigated the character type or characterization of the primary characters. The majority were adults ( , 36.8%) who were not doctors or scientists, followed by children ( , 28.1%), doctors ( , 21.1%), scientists ( , 5.2%), muppet characters ( , 5.2%), and coronavirus characters ( , 3.5%) (see Table 2 ).
RQ1b investigated whether the primary role model characters demonstrated health protection behaviors. The number of instances of health protection behaviors demonstrated by primary characters are as follows: 16 (22.9%) coughing into your arm, 15 (21.9%) washing hands, 14 (20.0%), wearing a face mask, 7 (10.0%), using hand sanitizer, 7 (10.0%), distancing from others, and 11 (15.7%) demonstrating other preventive health behaviors (see Table 3 ). Only 30.9% of videos featured a primary role model character who demonstrated health protection behaviors and 69.1% of videos featured primary role model character who did not demonstrate health protection behaviors. Some of the primary characters featured in the videos demonstrated multiple health protection behaviors. It is important to note that this analysis did not consider whether other characters appearing in the videos also demonstrated health protection behaviors.
4.3 Expert characters
RQ2 investigated if children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 featured expert characters ( medical or health professionals and scientists ). The first-appearing expert character was coded. 2 A total of 52 (38.2%) videos featured medical or health professionals (doctors, nurses, public health officials, psychiatrists) and 23 (16.9%) videos featured scientists as expert characters.
RQ2a examined the gender of the first-appearing expert characters. As reported in Table 4 , the gender of the first-appearing expert medical or health professional shown in the videos was 27 (51.9%) men, 19 (36.5%) women, and 6 (11.5%) gender could not be determined. The gender of the first-appearing scientist in the videos was 10 (43.5%) women, 8 (34.8%) men, and 5 (21.7%) gender not determined. 3
RQ2b investigated the race/ethnicity of the first-appearing expert characters. As reported in Table 5 , the race/ethnicity of the first-appearing expert medical or health professional was determined to be 31 (59.6%) white, 7 (13.5%) Black or African American, 2 (3.8%) Asian, 1 (1.9%) Hispanic or Latinx, and 11 (21.2%) as race/ethnicity could not be determined. An analysis of the race/ethnicity of the first-appearing scientist in the videos was determined to be 9 (39.1%) white, 5 (21.7%) Black or African American, 1 (4.4%) Asian, and 1 (4.4%) Hispanic or Latinx, with another 7 (30.4%) whose race/ethnicity could not be determined (see Table 5 ).
4.4 Coronavirus characters
RQ3 focused on the ways in which coronavirus characters were portrayed on the videos. The content analysis of the expressions on the faces of all-appearing coronavirus characters in the videos showed that most coronavirus characters had neutral expressions, followed in frequency by those who had scary or frightening expressions. Of all 103 coronavirus characters featured in the YouTube videos, 61 (59.2%) had neutral expressions, 20.4 (21%) had scary or frightening expressions, 10 (9.7%) had frowning or unhappy expressions, 8 (7.8%) had happy expressions, and 3 (2.9%) did not show any specific emotional expression.
4.5 Discourse frames
RQ4 focused on exploring the types of discourse frames ( informative , persuasive , directive ) used in the videos. Results showed that the directive frame ( ) represented half (50.0%), the informative frame ( ) reflected about one-third (34.0%), and the persuasive frame ( ) constituted over one-tenth (16%) of the 1,128 discourse frames analyzed.
RQ5 examined whether there were differences in the use of discourse frames for videos created before the CDC issued guidelines for communicating about COVID-19 to children and for videos created after these guidelines were released.
Differences in the use of directive frames were considered for the two different time periods. Descriptive analyses noted an increase in the percentage of discourse frames that used protective statements and decreases in the percentages of discourse frame statements about health habits and self-care for videos appearing after CDC guidelines were issued (Table 6 ).
As noted in Table 7 , differences in the use of informative frames were also found. The percentages of discourse frames that featured statements about the origin of the virus, symptoms, and inconveniences decreased in videos appearing after the CDC guidelines were issued. However, the percentage of the discourse frame that included statements about transmission of the virus doubled in videos appearing after these guidelines were released.
Descriptive analyses of the use of persuasive frames indicated changes in the percentages of all types of persuasive statements for videos appearing after the guidelines (Table 8 ). For example, the percentages of statements expressing power, fear, and reassurance all decreased while the use of statements expressing guilt and responsibility increased in videos appearing after the guidelines were released.
5 Discussion
Overall, this study provided valuable information about portrayals of primary characters as role models in digital media and the type of message framing used in children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 during the fight against transmission of COVID-19. The sudden emergence of COVID-19 as a public health crisis presented a unique communication challenge that required promoting an understanding of the disease for children and encouraging behavioral change to slow and prevent transmission of the virus. Effective communication of science, health, and medical information to children is a complex but critical and often overlooked area of research. The importance of research in this area is further underscored by the growing popularity of social media platforms, such as YouTube, as a source of science and health information [Brossard and Scheufele, 2013 ] and as a preferred medium among children [Jaakkola, 2020 ; Smith, Toor and van Kessel, 2018 ]. This exploratory study advanced extant research in this area by examining attributes of primary characters and the extent to which these characters demonstrated and modeled recommended health protection behavior, the diverse representation of expert characters, and the messaging approaches used in children’s YouTube videos to convey scientific, health, and medical information during a global pandemic.
Findings from the current study showed that the primary characters appearing in children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19 were most often adults. While approximately an equal number of primary characters were men and women, most primary characters were white and few were from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds. Portrayals of adult characters as role models in these videos are important because social learning research has found that children learn from adult media models, especially when children perceive adult models as nurturing and to be in high power positions [Bandura, 1963 ]. However, the lack of primary role models from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds is of concern because prior research has found that same-race role models are important for identification [Liss, 1981 ] and because of the reported health disparities, including hospitalization rates, from COVID-19, which were disproportionally higher for Black and Hispanic children in many U.S. states [Kim et al., 2020 ].
This study showed that many of the primary characters demonstrated and modeled health protection behaviors such as hand washing, coughing into their arms, wearing masks, and using hand sanitizer. However, more than twice as many videos did not feature a primary character demonstrating health protection behaviors than those that did. It is important to note that this study did not consider whether other characters appearing in the videos demonstrated protective health behaviors Social cognitive theory predicts that modeling of desired behavior exhibited by media models is likely to promote that behavior among children [Bandura, 1963 ; Bandura, Ross and Ross, 1963b ]. In line with prior research on children’s learning from television programs, it is expected that children also would learn to model the behavior of role model characters appearing in the YouTube videos. A meta-analysis of 24 studies found that children’s viewing of Sesame Street was associated with learning about “health and safety-related practices such as washing one’s hands or wearing a bike helmet” [Mares and Pan, 2013 , p. 148].
Research findings indicated that adult expert characters ( medical or health professionals and scientists ) appeared to some extent in these videos about COVID-19. Most of the videos, however, did not feature expert characters. Adult expert characters depicted as doctors and other medical professionals were featured much more often than adult expert characters depicted as scientists. Some adult expert characters demonstrated and modeled health protection behaviors such as hand washing, coughing into an elbow, or wearing a mask. Media models portrayed as medical or health experts may be important for effective communication to promote adoption of health protection behaviors through identificatory learning [Bandura, 1963 ], and thus potentially, shape children’s perceptions of and attitudes when important public health issues emerge. Other studies suggest that children are likely identify with experts, like those shown in these videos, when children perceive these characters as being in positions of high power [Bandura, 1963 ].
Findings from this study related to diverse portrayals of adult expert medical or health professional characters and scientist characters were mixed. Women were less likely to be represented as medical or health experts on these YouTube videos compared with their representation in the U.S. workforce. Findings from this study indicated that more men (51.9%) than women (36.5%) were depicted as expert medical or health professionals. Of all employed in health-related occupations in the actual U.S. workforce, 69.7% are female and 30.3% are male [National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics, 2021 ]. Interestingly, findings from this study found more women scientists (43.5%) than men scientists (34.8%) shown in these videos. Of all employed scientists and engineers in the actual U.S. workforce, 70.6% are male and 29.4% are female [National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics, 2021 ]. For both expert medical or health professional and scientist character portrayals, the representation of groups that have experienced exclusion and discrimination in the medical or health and STEM workforce were underrepresented in proportions similar to their underrepresentation in the actual U.S. workforce. Findings from this study on the race or ethnicity of the first-appearing expert medical or health professional indicated 31 (59.6%) white, 7 (13.5%) Black or African American, 2 (3.8%) Asian, 1 (1.9%) Hispanic or Latino, and 11 (21.2%) whose race or ethnicity could not be determined. For all ethnicities and races employed in health-related, occupations in the U.S., 67.9% were white, 11.8% were Black or African American, 9.2% were Asian, 9.2% were Hispanic or Latino, and 2.0% were more than one race, unknown, or other [National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics, 2021 ]. Findings from this study on the race or ethnicity of the first-appearing scientist in the videos indicated 9 (39.1%) were white, 5 (21.7%) were Black or African American, 1 (4.4%) was Asian, and 1 (4.4%) was Hispanic or Latinx, with another 7 (30.4%) whose race/ethnicity could not be determined. For all ethnicities and races employed in STEM occupations in the U.S., 64.5% were white, 18.1% were Asian, 8.1% were Hispanic or Latino, 7.4% were Black or African American, and 2.0% were more than one race, unknown, or other [National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics, 2021 ]. Overall, these findings call for more diverse and inclusive gender as well as racial and ethnic diversity in portrayals of adult expert characters to best promote identification for children of diverse backgrounds. Prior research has highlighted the importance of same-gender and same-race and ethnicity role models in promoting positive behavior among children [Carsten Conner and Danielson, 2016 ; Villiers et al., 2015 ].
Findings also revealed that animated anthropomorphic depictions of the coronavirus appeared in YouTube children’s videos about COVID-19. While most of the coronavirus anthropomorphisms were shown with a neutral expression, the next most common expression shown was a scary or frightening expression. The influence of visual representations on emotions has been noted in prior studies [Powell et al., 2015 ], and this influence can also impact and motivate behavior [Krause and Bucy, 2018 ]. Prior research has found that anthropomorphic depictions promote children’s learning from media [Bonus and Mares, 2018 ]. However, other research on anthropomorphic depictions of coronavirus in newspaper cartoons noted that these visual representations convey “broader anxieties, fears and (mis)conceptions” [Joubert and Wasserman, 2020 , p. 20] that shape negatively public attitudes and perceptions. Further, the formal features (bright colors, abrupt movement) exhibited by many of these anthropomorphic depictions of the coronavirus may attract children’s attention — either towards or away from important health protective messaging — and potentially making these depictions more salient for and having a greater or less influence on young viewers.
Findings from the current study show how language conveyed through discourse frames about COVID-19 was used to urgently communicate important social rules and expected norms of behavior to children in order to combat this major public health crisis. The three discourse frames examined in this study were manifested differently in the YouTube videos and varied in their frequency of appearance. Language has been described not only as a communicatory vehicle but also as a social practice that conveys and reinforces “tacitly understood rules and forms of behaviour which regulate ordinary life practices” [Taylor, 2013 , p. 11]. As one researcher explained: “The words and terms used in various contexts, officially and colloquially, are part of the chain; the language is inseparable from the social phenomenon” [Taylor, 2013 , p. 9].
The use of the directive discourse frame appeared to be adopted most frequently to communicate health information, in particular, related to promoting personal and public health. Direct statements about ways to prevent transmission of the virus were frequent such as those found in the video by Healthwise [ 2020 ]: “Stay home to avoid people who might be infected”. “Avoid touching your mouth, nose, and eyes”. “Stay home when you are sick and ask people not to visit you”. There are several possible explanations for the frequent use of the directive frame. First, directive discourse frames mirror a frequent parent-to-child communicative style when parents are concerned about children’s safety and well-being, and especially for communication with young children. Second, directive discourse frames present information in a clear and straight-forward manner that is most likely to be most assessible and easy for children to understand because information is conveyed in a simple, concise, and clear manner. Third, directive discourse frames convey authority and certainty.
Prior research has suggested that direct forms of communication may be more compelling, noting that “scientists need to be affirmative when communicating about scientific issues: polite and vague message styles may make scientists look weak and vacillating” [Yuan, Ma and Besley, 2019 , p. 285]. The predominant use of directive discourse frames, thus, could also be related to the need for direct communication about the severity of the pandemic. Importantly, the percentage of protective statements in videos appearing after the CDC announced guidelines on how to discuss the virus with children increased. This may underscore the sense of urgency needed to communicate the importance of protecting children from contracting the virus and then transmitting the virus to others in their family. Such transmission could be especially worrisome for older adults (e.g., grandparents), individuals with chronic conditions, and those who are immunocompromised.
Although less frequent, informative discourse frames also were found in children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19. Informative discourse frames focused on facts about the coronavirus such as those provided in the video by High Impact [ 2020 ]: “COVID-19 is believed to be transmitted primarily through inhalation”. “Once infected most people experience, mild symptoms including sore throat, headache, fever, a dry cough, shortness of breath, and fatigue”. Informative frames may be especially critical for promoting children’s learning and understanding of complex information about the virus, transmission, symptoms, and treatment. This seems especially relevant for videos after the CDC issued its COVID-19 communication guidelines targeting children. However, findings showed that the percentages of statements about COVID-19 origins, symptoms, and inconveniences decreased while the percentage of statements about transmission increased in videos appearing after the guidelines were issued. While a large number of videos both before and after issuance of the guidelines discussed transmission, this finding highlights an increase in the percentage of videos featuring statements about transmission after the guidelines were released. Overall, this indicated that these videos focused even more on the need for children to physically distance from others in order to protect themselves and others from contracting and transmitting the virus.
Persuasive discourse frames were the least likely discourse frame to appear in these videos. However, persuasive framing may be important for understanding motivation for adopting protective health measures. Persuasive discourse frames varied in the type of appeal. Some videos featured reassuring discourse frames like the frame in the video by BayCare [ 2020 ]: “Wearing a mask is one way that we can help keep everyone safe”. Others featured fear-inducing discourse frames like the frame used in video by Healthwise [ 2020 ]: “COVID-19 is new and has not been seen in people before” and “For some people it can become serious and dangerous”. Interestingly, the use of both the fear-inducing and reassuring frames significantly decreased, after the CDC published its guidelines for communicating with children about COVID-19. It appears that less emphasis on the threat caused by the virus combined with statements expressing confidence in overcoming that threat may be a good combination in helping children understand the serious nature of the pandemic without conveying additional fear or unrealistic assurance. Prior research has found fear appeals effective in promoting positive health behaviors about food consumption [Bleakley et al., 2015 ; Charry and Demoulin, 2012 ]. However, other research has found that message framing focused outcomes (gain vs. loss) and message polarity (affirmation vs. negation) were important considerations when developing Public Service Announcements (PSAs) for children designed to promote behavioral changes [Wyllie, Baxter and Kulczynski, 2015 ]. To date, little research has considered the most effective discourse frames for communication of scientific and health information to children.
5.1 Study limitations and future research
The potential influence of role model characters and discourse frames on science and health communication for children is an important and understudied area of research. This exploratory study addressed this gap and provided a foundation for future research on media content and media effects in this area. This study provided useful information and an interesting framework for better understanding science, health, and medical communication to children; however, there were several limitations of this research.
First, the sample size was relatively small. During the sampling timeframe, there were relatively few COVID-19 videos specifically created for children posted on YouTube. Future research could consider how other media communicated information about COVID-19 to children to better determine how multiple media sources conveyed information about the virus and protective health behaviors during the pandemic. Second, the overall sample of videos analyzed for this study was limited by the search terms used to identify the videos. However, multiple keyword combinations and terms were used (e.g., kids, teens) to obtain as large and as comprehensive a sample of YouTube children’s videos about COVID-19 as possible. In addition, an even broader list of search terms for specific health protection behaviors was used in the second sampling of videos. Third, while this content analysis offered valuable baseline data for assessing portrayals of role model characters and discourse frames, future research should also consider other variables considered to be effective for science and health communication with children. Fourth, it is important to extend this research through audience studies that assess the actual effects of viewing these YouTube videos of COVID-19 on children’s attitudes and behaviors. It is critical to assess which primary characters (including expert and anthropomorphic coronavirus characters) are most effective in communicating risks and promoting the adoption of health protection behaviors. In addition, it is important to determine which specific messaging strategies and discourse frames are most effective for science and health communication for children. While it was beyond the scope of this study to provide information about the effects of these videos on the target audience, these effects are important to assess. Similarly, while an analysis of production goals and conventions also were beyond the scope of the current study, future research should consider their impact on messaging content and design.
Despite these limitations, providing descriptive information about the content of these videos related to the characters featured and messaging approaches used is an important first step. To extend research with this focus, future research, for example, could further explore the effects of role model portrayals, role model demonstration of health protection behaviors, and discourse frames on children’s understanding of the science, health, and medical content and on the adoption of health protection behaviors. The variety of discourse frames used to communicate information about COVID-19 to children during this global pandemic underscores the need to examine the effects of these discourse frames on children. Future audience research assessing the effects of specific types of discourse frames in science videos for children could help determine the most effective ways of presenting science, health, and medical information to promote attention, engagement, learning, recall, and understanding. Additional work could also consider whether different types of discourses (informative, directive, persuasive) are more influential than others or more developmentally appropriate than others, especially given the wide range of ages targeted in children’s YouTube videos about COVID-19. The persuasive frames that emphasized social responsibility as a form of persuasion is another important area of research. For example, one previous study found that descriptive social-norm based health messages and those messages highlighting the healthy behaviors of others had a small effect on promoting healthy behavior related to eating fruits and vegetables [Sharps and Robinson, 2016 ].
Overall, additional research is needed on how children process information from YouTube videos about science, health, and medicine. What features are most engaging? What elements in these videos do children focus on? How do children process scientific, health, and medical information presented in the videos? Which discourse frames are most effective and for which health outcomes? How do children’s interpretation vary by age? Future audience effects research with children also should examine analytics data on video viewers and consider individual differences in viewers’ overall use, amount of time viewing, motivation for watching, attention span, and viewing context (formal or informal or blended), in addition to viewer’s identities and demographics. Finally, given the prevalence of animated coronaviruses in these videos, further investigation of their potential effects should be considered. Prior research has stressed the importance of considering the influence of visual content on understanding of scientific issues, noting that visuals may be more influential than text [Powell et al., 2015 ].
6 Conclusion
The COVID-19 pandemic highlighted an urgent need to understand how to accurately and effectively communicate complex scientific and health information to young learners through media. The COVID-19 pandemic represents one of the most significant public health crises worldwide in recent years. As the pandemic evolved, initial concerns about children transmitting the virus to adults shifted to increased concerns about children contracting, being hospitalized, and dying from the virus. In the first two years of the pandemic, 296 children ages 0–4 and 644 children ages 5–18 died from COVID-19 in the U.S., according to the Centers for Disease Control (CDC), [Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2022 ]. While the development and approval of vaccines helped decrease the spread of the virus, effective communication to children about COVID-19 protective behaviors remained critical. Throughout the COVID-19 pandemic, YouTube was one likely source of information about the virus for children as well as a potentially useful medium for educating children about the virus.
This exploratory study provided important foundational knowledge about the media content and messaging in YouTube videos about COVID-19 created for children, focusing on the use of characterizations of role models and discourse frames appearing in these videos. Findings from this study identified YouTube video content related to the selection of the characters who served as spokespersons and also as role models. Many of these characters demonstrated health protection behaviors as well as provided information to educate children about the coronavirus. Findings from this study also highlighted how the use of anthropomorphic coronavirus characters were used to teach children what to think about the virus and also how to think about the virus through the facial expressions that conveyed emotion in these character depictions. In addition, study findings examined the use of various discourse frames used in communicating science, health, and medical information to children, highlighting how specific messages and values were embedded in the messages used to communicate about COVID-19 with children. Finally, the YouTube videos included in this study targeted a wide range of ages from the preschool years to adolescence.
This exploratory study also identified several important messaging features critical for understanding the potential effects of science, health, and medical communication YouTube videos on children’s attitudes and behavioral change. Findings from this exploratory research study helped contribute to efforts to improve communication with children through digital media and offered important avenues for future research designed to better understand the potential influence of digital media content on young viewers. However, many questions and avenues for future research in this area remain. For example, future research should consider which characters (teachers, parents, doctors or other health professionals, scientists, peers, or others) are most effective role models in producing the desired health behavioral outcomes. The efficacy of matched gender and matched race and ethnicity role models is another important consideration. It is important for future research in this area to consider carefully how live-action and animated characters may vary in their appeal to children. In addition, it is important to consider how representations of anthropomorphic characters serve as visualizations and metaphors that affect children’s emotions and shape their perceptions of health concerns. Future research needs to determine which types of discourse frames are most effective and most persuasive in urging children’s adoption of protective health behaviors.
Digital media, like YouTube videos, are likely to appeal to many children because of the characters, engaging storytelling features, and action-focused nature of the medium. The growing popularity of YouTube videos among young viewers and their increasing access to YouTube videos underscore the importance of future research on their effects. Further, the increasing number of educators and practitioners creating YouTube videos as educational tools to convey information about science, health, and medicine emphasizes the need for more research on both its reach and effects. Educating children about public health issues is important, but the communication of scientific, health, and medical information to children is a challenging task. Scientific, health, and medical information is complex, and children and youth are active, not passive, users of popular media, and thus likely to engage differently with YouTube characters and YouTube message content. Additional research will help fill this gap in understanding how to most effectively create science, health, and medical content in digital media to best promote attitudinal and behavioral change, for this important, and often overlooked, audience.
Appendix A. YouTube children’s videos about COVID-19 (February–September, 2020)
References
-
Adnsager, J. L., Austin, E. W. and Pinkleton, B. E. (2001). ‘Questioning the value of realism: young adults’ processing of messages in alcohol-related public service announcements and advertising’. Journal of Communication 51 (1), pp. 121–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2001.tb02875.x .
-
Allgaier, J. (2013). ‘On the shoulders of YouTube: science in music videos’. Science Communication 35 (2), pp. 266–275. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547012454949 .
-
Anderson, A. A., Brossard, D. and Scheufele, D. A. (2010). ‘The changing information environment for nanotechnology: online audiences and content’. Journal of Nanoparticle Research 12 (4), pp. 1083–1094. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11051-010-9860-2 .
-
Anderson, M. and Jiang, J. (2018). Teens, social media and technology 2018 . Washington, DC, U.S.A.: Pew Research Center. URL: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2018/05/31/teens-social-media-technology-2018/ .
-
Bandura, A. (1963). ‘Behavior theory and identificatory learning’. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 33 (4), pp. 591–601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1963.tb01007.x .
-
— (1969). ‘Social-learning theory of identificatory processes’. In: Handbook of socialization theory and research. Ed. by D. A. Goslin. Chicago, IL, U.S.A.: Rand McNally & Company, pp. 213–262.
-
— (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, U.S.A.: Prentice-Hall.
-
— (2001). ‘Social cognitive theory of mass communication’. Media Psychology 3 (3), pp. 265–299. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532785xmep0303_03 .
-
— (2002). ‘Social cognitive theory of mass communication’. In: Media effects: advances in theory and research. Ed. by J. Bryant and D. Zillmann. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ, U.S.A.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 121–153.
-
— (2009). ‘Social cognitive theory of mass communication’. In: Media effects: advances in theory and research. Ed. by J. Bryant and M. B. Oliver. 3rd ed. New York, NY, U.S.A.: Routledge, pp. 94–124. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203877111 .
-
Bandura, A., Ross, D. and Ross, S. A. (1963a). ‘A comparative test of the status envy, social power, and secondary reinforcement theories of identificatory learning’. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67 (6), pp. 527–534. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046546 .
-
— (1963b). ‘Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models’. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66 (1), pp. 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048687 .
-
BayCare (2020). For kids: why do I have to wear a face mask? URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vGB-hxst7fo .
-
Bleakley, A., Jordan, A. B., Hennessy, M., Glanz, K., Strasser, A. and Vaala, S. (2015). ‘Do emotional appeals in public service advertisements influence adolescents’ intention to reduce consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages?’ Journal of Health Communication 20 (8), pp. 938–948. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2015.1018593 .
-
Bonus, J. A. and Mares, M.-L. (2018). ‘When the sun sings science, are children left in the dark? Representations of science in children’s television and their effects on children’s learning’. Human Communication Research 44 (4), pp. 449–472. https://doi.org/10.1093/hcr/hqy009 .
-
Brodin, P. (2020). ‘Why is COVID-19 so mild in children?’ Acta Paediatrica 109 (6), pp. 1082–1083. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.15271 .
-
Brossard, D. and Scheufele, D. A. (2013). ‘Science, new media, and the public’. Science 339 (6115), pp. 40–41. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232329 .
-
Buonsenso, D., Sali, M., Pata, D., De Rose, C., Sanguinetti, M., Valentini, P. and Delogu, G. (2020). ‘Children and COVID-19: microbiological and immunological insights’. Pediatric Pulmonology 55 (10), pp. 2547–2555. https://doi.org/10.1002/ppul.24978 .
-
Cacciatore, M. A., Scheufele, D. A. and Iyengar, S. (2016). ‘The end of framing as we know it … and the future of media effects’. Mass Communication and Society 19 (1), pp. 7–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2015.1068811 .
-
Carsten Conner, L. D. and Danielson, J. (2016). ‘Scientist role models in the classroom: how important is gender matching?’ International Journal of Science Education 38 (15), pp. 2414–2430. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2016.1246780 .
-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (20th May 2020). Talking with children about Coronavirus Disease 2019 .
-
— (2022). Provisional COVID-19 deaths: focus on ages 0–18 years . URL: https://data.cdc.gov/widgets/nr4s-juj3?mobile_redirect=true .
-
Charry, K. M. and Demoulin, N. T. M. (2012). ‘Behavioural evidence for the effectiveness of threat appeals in the promotion of healthy food to children’. International Journal of Advertising 31 (4), pp. 773–794. https://doi.org/10.2501/ija-31-4-773-794 .
-
Cinelli, M., Quattrociocchi, W., Galeazzi, A., Valensise, C. M., Brugnoli, E., Schmidt, A. L., Zola, P., Zollo, F. and Scala, A. (2020). ‘The COVID-19 social media infodemic’. Scientific Reports 10, 16598. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-73510-5 .
-
Cluver, L., Lachman, J. M., Sherr, L., Wessels, I., Krug, E., Rakotomalala, S., Blight, S., Hillis, S., Bachman, G., Green, O., Butchart, A., Tomlinson, M., Ward, C. L., Doubt, J. and McDonald, K. (2020). ‘Parenting in a time of COVID-19’. The Lancet 395 (10231), E64. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30736-4 .
-
Delahoy, M. J., Ujamaa, D., Whitaker, M., O’Halloran, A., Anglin, O., Burns, E., Cummings, C., Holstein, R., Kambhampati, A. K., Milucky, J., Patel, K., Pham, H., Taylor, C. A., Chai, S. J., Reingold, A., Alden, N. B., Kawasaki, B., Meek, J., Yousey-Hindes, K., Anderson, E. J., Openo, K. P., Teno, K., Weigel, A., Kim, S., Leegwater, L., Bye, E., Como-Sabetti, K., Ropp, S., Rudin, D., Muse, A., Spina, N., Bennett, N. M., Popham, K., Billing, L. M., Shiltz, E., Sutton, M., Thomas, A., Schaffner, W., Keipp Talbot, H., Crossland, M. T., McCaffrey, K., Hall, A. J., Fry, A. M., McMorrow, M., Reed, C., Garg, S. and Havers, F. P. (2021). ‘Hospitalizations associated with COVID-19 among children and adolescents — COVID-NET, 14 States, March 1, 2020–August 14, 2021’. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) 70 (36), pp. 1255–1260. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm7036e2 .
-
Detenber, B. H., Ho, S. S., Ong, A. H. and Lim, N. W. B. (2018). ‘Complementary versus competitive framing effects in the context of pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors’. Science Communication 40 (2), pp. 173–198. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018758075 .
-
Entman, R. M. (1993). ‘Framing: toward clarification of a fractured paradigm’. Journal of Communication 43 (4), pp. 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x .
-
Everhart, J. (2009). ‘YouTube in the science classroom’. Science and Children 46 (9), pp. 32–35.
-
Farrelly, M. C., Niederdeppe, J. and Yarsevich, J. (2003). ‘Youth tobacco prevention mass media campaigns: past, present, and future directions’. Tobacco Control 12 (suppl 1), pp. i35–i47. https://doi.org/10.1136/tc.12.suppl_1.i35 .
-
Foucault, M. (1972). The archaeology of knowledge. New York, NY, U.S.A.: Pantheon Books.
-
Freelon, D. (2010). ‘ReCal: intercoder reliability calculation as a web service’. International Journal of Internet Science 5 (1), pp. 20–33. URL: http://dfreelon.org/utils/recalfront/ .
-
Gamson, W. A. (1989). ‘News as framing’. American Behavioral Scientist 33 (2), pp. 157–161. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764289033002006 .
-
Gamson, W. A. and Modigliani, A. (1989). ‘Media discourse and public opinion on nuclear power: a constructionist approach’. American Journal of Sociology 95 (1), pp. 1–37. https://doi.org/10.1086/229213 .
-
García, E. and Weiss, E. (2020). COVID-19 and student performance, equity, and U.S. education policy . Washington, DC, U.S.A.: Economic Policy Institute. URL: https://www.epi.org/publication/the-consequences-of-the-covid-19-pandemic-for-education-performance-and-equity-in-the-united-states-what-can-we-learn-from-pre-pandemic-research-to-inform-relief-recovery-and-rebuilding/ .
-
Gelmez Burakgazi, S. and Yildirim, A. (2014). ‘Accessing science through media: uses and gratifications among fourth and fifth graders for science learning’. Science Communication 36 (2), pp. 168–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547013505847 .
-
Grootens-Wiegers, P., De Vries, M. C., Vossen, T. E. and Van den Broek, J. M. (2015). ‘Readability and visuals in medical research information forms for children and adolescents’. Science Communication 37 (1), pp. 89–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547014558942 .
-
Healthwise (2020). What is COVID 19 . URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4pVXILBOsHQ .
-
High Impact (2020). Coronavirus animation: high impact demonstrates how COVID-19 impacts the body . URL: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=78jLBNSqc3g .
-
Hoffner, C. and Buchanan, M. (2005). ‘Young adults’ wishful identification with television characters: the role of perceived similarity and character attributes’. Media Psychology 7 (4), pp. 325–351. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532785xmep0704_2 .
-
Hourcade, J. P., Mascher, S. L., Wu, D. and Pantoja, L. (2015). ‘Look, my baby is using an iPad! An analysis of YouTube videos of infants and toddlers using tablets’. In: CHI ’15: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (Seoul, Republic of Korea, 18th–23rd April 2015). New York, NY, U.S.A.: Association for Computing Machinery, pp. 1915–1924. https://doi.org/10.1145/2702123.2702266 .
-
Hunter, P. (2020). ‘The growth of social media in science: social media has evolved from a mere communication channel to an integral tool for discussion and research collaboration’. EMBO Reports 21 (5), e50550. https://doi.org/10.15252/embr.202050550 .
-
Huttunen, S. and Hildén, M. (2014). ‘Framing the controversial: geoengineering in academic literature’. Science Communication 36 (1), pp. 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547013492435 .
-
Iyengar, S. (1989). ‘How citizens think about national issues: a matter of responsibility’. American Journal of Political Science 33 (4), pp. 878–900. https://doi.org/10.2307/2111113 .
-
— (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago, IL, U.S.A.: University of Chicago Press.
-
Jaakkola, M. (2020). ‘From vernacularized commercialism to kidbait: toy review videos on YouTube and the problematics of the mash-up genre’. Journal of Children and Media 14 (2), pp. 237–254. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2019.1693409 .
-
Jones, T. and Cuthrell, K. (2011). ‘YouTube: educational potentials and pitfalls’. Computers in the Schools 28 (1), pp. 75–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/07380569.2011.553149 .
-
Jones-Jang, S. M., Hart, P. S., Feldman, L. and Moon, W.-K. (2020). ‘Diversifying or reinforcing science communication? Examining the flow of frame contagion across media platforms’. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 97 (1), pp. 98–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699019874731 .
-
Joubert, M. and Wasserman, H. (2020). ‘Spikey blobs with evil grins: understanding portrayals of the coronavirus in South African newspaper cartoons in relation to the public communication of science’. JCOM 19 (07), A08. https://doi.org/10.22323/2.19070208 .
-
Kang, S., Ha, J.-S. and Velasco, T. (2017). ‘Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on YouTube: framing, anchoring, and objectification in social media’. Community Mental Health Journal 53 (4), pp. 445–451. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-016-0015-5 .
-
Kim, L., Whitaker, M., O’Halloran, A., Kambhampati, A., Chai, S. J., Reingold, A., Armistead, I., Kawasaki, B., Meek, J., Yousey-Hindes, K., Anderson, E. J., Openo, K. P., Weigel, A., Ryan, P., Monroe, M. L., Fox, K., Kim, S., Lynfield, R., Bye, E., Davis, S. S., Smelser, C., Barney, G., Spina, N. L., Bennett, N. M., Felsen, C. B., Billing, L. M., Shiltz, J., Sutton, M., West, N., Talbot, H. K., Schaffner, W., Risk, I., Price, A., Brammer, L., Fry, A. M., Hall, A. J., Langley, G. E. and Garg, S. (2020). ‘Hospitalization rates and characteristics of children aged years hospitalized with laboratory-confirmed COVID-19 — COVID-NET, 14 States, March 1–July 25, 2020’. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) 69 (32), pp. 1081–1088. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6932e3 .
-
Krause, A. and Bucy, E. P. (2018). ‘Interpreting images of fracking: how visual frames and standing attitudes shape perceptions of environmental risk and economic benefit’. Environmental Communication 12 (3), pp. 322–343. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1412996 .
-
Liss, M. B. (1981). ‘Children’s television selections: a study of indicators of same-race preferences’. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 12 (1), pp. 103–110. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022181121008 .
-
Lombard, M., Snyder-Duch, J. and Bracken, C. C. (2002). ‘Content analysis in mass communication: assessment and reporting of intercoder reliability’. Human Communication Research 28 (4), pp. 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x .
-
Ludvigsson, J. F. (2020). ‘Children are unlikely to be the main drivers of the COVID-19 pandemic — A systematic review’. Acta Paediatrica 109 (8), pp. 1525–1530. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.15371 .
-
Mares, M.-L., Cantor, J. and Steinbach, J. B. (1999). ‘Using television to foster children’s interest in science’. Science Communication 20 (3), pp. 283–297. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547099020003001 .
-
Mares, M.-L. and Pan, Z. (2013). ‘Effects of Sesame Street : a meta-analysis of children’s learning in 15 countries’. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 34 (3), pp. 140–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2013.01.001 .
-
Marsh, J., Plowman, L., Yamada-Rice, D., Bishop, J. C., Lahmar, J., Scott, F., Davenport, A., Davis, S., French, K., Piras, M., Thornhill, S., Robinson, P. and Winter, P. (2015). Exploring play and creativity in pre-schoolers’ use of apps: report for early years practitioners . URL: http://www.techandplay.org .
-
McCauley, M., Minsky, S. and Viswanath, K. (2013). ‘The H1N1 pandemic: media frames, stigmatization and coping’. BMC Public Health 13, 1116. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-1116 .
-
Miri, S. M., Noorbakhsh, F., Mohebbi, S. R. and Ghaemi, A. (2020). ‘Higher prevalence of asymptomatic or mild COVID-19 in children, claims and clues’. Journal of Medical Virology 92 (11), pp. 2257–2259. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmv.26069 .
-
Moyer-Gusé, E. (2008). ‘Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education messages’. Communication Theory 18 (3), pp. 407–425. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x .
-
National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics (2021). Women, minorities, and persons with disabilities in science and engineering . Alexandria, VA, U.S.A.: National Science Foundation. URL: https://ncses.nsf.gov/wmpd .
-
Neumann, M. M. and Herodotou, C. (2020a). ‘Evaluating YouTube videos for young children’. Education and Information Technologies 25 (5), pp. 4459–4475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-020-10183-7 .
-
— (2020b). ‘Young children and YouTube: a global phenomenon’. Childhood Education 96 (4), pp. 72–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2020.1796459 .
-
Nicolini, V., Cassia, F. and Bellotto, M. (2017). ‘Children perceptions of emotional and rational appeals in social advertisements’. Young Consumers 18 (3), pp. 261–277. https://doi.org/10.1108/YC-02-2017-00665 .
-
Oterino Serrano, C., Alonso, E., Andrés, M., Buitrago, N. M., Pérez Vigara, A., Parrón Pajares, M., Cuesta López, E., Garzón Moll, G., Martin Espin, I., Bueno Barriocanal, M., De Ceano-Vivas la Calle, M., Calvo Rey, C. and Bret-Zurita, M. (2020). ‘Pediatric chest x-ray in covid-19 infection’. European Journal of Radiology 131, 109236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejrad.2020.109236 .
-
Papadopoulos, L. (2018). ‘7 female science YouTubers that are breaking STEM glass ceilings’. Interesting Engineering . URL: https://interestingengineering.com/7-female-science-youtubers-that-are-breaking-stem-glass-ceilings .
-
Pinkleton, B. E., Austin, E. W. and Fujioka, Y. (2001). ‘The relationship of perceived beer ad and PSA quality to high school students’ alcohol-related beliefs and behaviors’. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 45 (4), pp. 575–597. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4504_3 .
-
Pinkleton, B. E., Austin, E. W. and Van de Vord, R. (2010). ‘The role of realism, similarity, and expectancies in adolescents’ interpretation of abuse-prevention messages’. Health Communication 25 (3), pp. 258–265. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410231003698937 .
-
Powell, T. E., Boomgaarden, H. G., De Swert, K. and de Vreese, C. H. (2015). ‘A clearer picture: the contribution of visuals and text to framing effects’. Journal of Communication 65 (6), pp. 997–1017. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12184 .
-
Renee, H. (2018). ‘8 women in STEM on YouTube you need to watch’. Shenova Fashion . URL: https://shenovafashion.com/blogs/blog/8-women-in-stem-on-youtube-you-need-to-watch-1 (visited on 19th November 2020).
-
Rhodes, N., Ralston, R. and Bigsby, E. (2016). ‘Teens’ reactance to anti-smoking public service announcements: how norms set the stage’. Journal of Health Communication 21 (5), pp. 575–582. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2015.1114052 .
-
Samson, L., Nanne, A. J. and Buijzen, M. (2021). ‘Remember the motivationally-relevant appeals? The influence of social and sensory appeals on memory for pronutritional messages promoting healthy foods’. International Journal of Advertising 40 (4), pp. 582–601. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2020.1833675 .
-
Scott, W. A. (1955). ‘Reliability of content analysis: the case of nominal scale coding’. Public Opinion Quarterly 19 (3), pp. 321–325. https://doi.org/10.1086/266577 .
-
Sharps, M. and Robinson, E. (2016). ‘Encouraging children to eat more fruit and vegetables: health vs. descriptive social norm-based messages’. Appetite 100, pp. 18–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.01.031 .
-
Siegel, D. A., Reses, H. E., Cool, A. J., Shapiro, C. N., Hsu, J., Boehmer, T. K., Cornwell, C. R., Gray, E. B., Henley, S. J., Lochner, K., Suthar, A. B., Lyons, B. C., Mattocks, L., Hartnett, K., Adjemian, J., van Santen, K. L., Sheppard, M., Soetebier, K. A., Logan, P., Martin, M., Idubor, O., Natarajan, P., Sircar, K., Oyegun, E., Dalton, J., Perrine, C. G., Peacock, G., Schweitzer, B., Morris, S. B. and Raizes, E. (2021). ‘Trends in COVID-19 cases, emergency department visits, and hospital admissions among children and adolescents aged 0–17 years — United States, August 2020–August 2021’. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) 70 (36), pp. 1249–1254. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm7036e1 .
-
Simis-Wilkinson, M., Madden, H., Lassen, D., Su, L. Y.-F., Brossard, D., Scheufele, D. A. and Xenos, M. A. (2018). ‘Scientists joking on social media: an empirical analysis of #overlyhonestmethods’. Science Communication 40 (3), pp. 314–339. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018766557 .
-
Slater, M. D., Rouner, D., Beauvais, F., Murphy, K., Domenech-Rodriguez, M. and Van Leuven, J. (1996). ‘Adolescent perceptions of underage drinkers in TV beer ads’. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education 42 (1), pp. 43–56. URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/45092139 .
-
Smith, A., Toor, S. and van Kessel, P. (2018). Many turn to YouTube for children’s content, news, how-to lessons . Washington, DC, U.S.A.: Pew Research Center. URL: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2018/11/07/many-turn-to-youtube-for-childrens-content-news-how-to-lessons/ (visited on 16th June 2020).
-
Smith, D. K. (2014). ‘iTube, YouTube, WeTube: social media videos in chemistry education and outreach’. Journal of Chemical Education 91 (10), pp. 1594–1599. https://doi.org/10.1021/ed400715s .
-
Steinke, J., Applegate, B., Lapinski, M., Ryan, L. and Long, M. (2012). ‘Gender differences in adolescents’ wishful identification with scientist characters on television’. Science Communication 34 (2), pp. 163–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547011410250 .
-
Stewart, C. O. (2013). ‘The influence of news frames and science background on attributions about embryonic and adult stem cell research: frames as heuristic/biasing cues’. Science Communication 35 (1), pp. 86–114. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547012440517 .
-
Tan, L., Ng, S. H., Omar, A. and Karupaiah, T. (2018). ‘What’s on YouTube? A case study on food and beverage advertising in videos targeted at children on social media’. Childhood Obesity 14 (5), pp. 280–290. https://doi.org/10.1089/chi.2018.0037 .
-
Taylor, S. (2013). ‘Theories and common concerns’. In: What is discourse analysis? London, U.K.: Bloomsbury Academic, pp. 7–28. https://doi.org/10.5040/9781472545213.ch-002 .
-
UNESCO (2020). Education: from disruption to recovery . URL: https://en.unesco.org/covid19/educationresponse (visited on 25th October 2021).
-
UNICEF (2020). COVID-19 and its implications for protecting children online . World Childhood Foundation, End Violence Against Children, ITU, UNESCO, UNODC, WePROTECT Global Alliance, WHO. URL: https://reliefweb.int/report/world/covid-19-and-its-implications-protecting-children-online-april-2020 .
-
van Kessel, P. (2019). 10 facts about Americans and YouTube . Washington, DC, U.S.A.: Pew Research Center. URL: https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/12/04/10-facts-about-americans-and-youtube/ (visited on 16th June 2020).
-
Villiers, C., Gilbert, S., Barringer, T. A., Hyatt, A. M., Horton, J. W. and Hinds, D. (2015). ‘Development of a STEM summer program for underrepresented high school students — A success story’. In: 2015 ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition (Seattle, WA, U.S.A. 14th–17th June 2015). https://doi.org/10.18260/p.23868 .
-
Vlach, H. A. and Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). ‘Distributing learning over time: the spacing effect in children’s acquisition and generalization of science concepts’. Child Development 83 (4), pp. 1137–1144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01781.x .
-
Wyllie, J., Baxter, S. and Kulczynski, A. (2015). ‘Healthy kids: examining the effect of message framing and polarity on children’s attitudes and behavioral intentions’. Journal of Advertising 44 (2), pp. 140–150. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2015.1018462 .
-
Xie, L. (2015). ‘The story of two big chimneys: a frame analysis of climate change in US and Chinese newspapers’. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research 44 (2), pp. 151–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/17475759.2015.1011593 .
-
Xue, J., Chen, J., Chen, C., Zheng, C., Li, S. and Zhu, T. (2020). ‘Public discourse and sentiment during the COVID 19 pandemic: using latent Dirichlet allocation for topic modeling on Twitter’. PLoS ONE 15 (9), e0239441. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239441 .
-
Yadav, S., Chakraborty, P., Mittal, P. and Arora, U. (2018). ‘Children aged 6–24 months like to watch YouTube videos but could not learn anything from them’. Acta Paediatrica 107 (8), pp. 1461–1466. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.14291 .
-
YouTube (2021). Content policies for YouTube Kids . URL: https://support.google.com/youtube/answer/10938174?hl=en&ref_topic=9282435 (visited on 30th November 2021).
-
Yuan, S., Ma, W. and Besley, J. C. (2019). ‘Should scientists talk about GMOs nicely? Exploring the effects of communication styles, source expertise, and preexisting attitude’. Science Communication 41 (3), pp. 267–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547019837623 .
-
Zhao, X. and Cai, X. (2016). ‘The association between exposure to “Tips” and smoking-related outcomes among adolescents in the United States’. Health Education Research 31 (5), pp. 614–623. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyw037 .
-
Zhu, Y., Bloxham, C. J., Hulme, K. D., Sinclair, J. E., Tong, Z. W. M., Steele, L. E., Noye, E. C., Lu, J., Xia, Y., Chew, K. Y., Pickering, J., Gilks, C., Bowen, A. C. and Short, K. R. (2021). ‘A meta-analysis on the role of children in severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 in household transmission clusters’. Clinical Infectious Diseases 72 (12), e1146–e1153. https://doi.org/10.1093/cid/ciaa1825 .
Authors
Jocelyn Steinke is an associate professor in the Department of Communication at the University of Connecticut. E-mail: jocelyn.steinke@uconn.edu .
Carolyn A. Lin is a professor in the Department of Communication at the University of Connecticut. E-mail: carolyn.lin@uconn.edu .
Tamia Duncan received her master’s degree from the School of Communication at Western Michigan University. E-mail: Tamia.Duncan@wmich.edu .
Viviana Zambrano is a doctoral student in the Department of Communication at the University of Connecticut. E-mail: vivianaczambrano@gmail.com .
Endnotes
1 None of the videos from the first reliability check were coded in the second reliability check.
2 Some, but not all, of the expert characters were primary characters in the videos.
3 The original research questions did not compare the number of men and women adult characters in expert roles ( medical or other health professionals , scientists ). However, a Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Test was conducted to determine whether the proportion of women medical or other health professional characters was equal to the proportion of men medical or other health professional characters. The proportions did not differ by gender, , . A Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Test was performed to determine whether the proportion of women scientist characters was equal to the proportion of men scientist characters. The proportions did not differ by gender, , . No significant statistical difference was found for either of these comparisons.