1 Context and objective

Although climate change is one of the defining topics of our time [Ki-moon in United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change, 2008], for many members of the public and policymakers alike, it is (still) not a major cause for concern [e.g., Bell, Poushter, Fagan & Huang, 2021; Carmichael, Brulle & Huxster, 2017]. Although global concern has grown since 2013, there are differences in levels of concern across countries. For instance, in Germany, 71% see climate change as a major threat but in the United States (US) and South Africa, this number sits at 59% [Fagan & Huang, 2019]. There are many reasons why climate change is not a top priority for some, and part of the reason identified by researchers is that climate change impacts [i.e., climate change-related future scenarios or climate futures; see Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022] are often perceived as psychologically distant. That means that they are not seen as personally affecting individuals, are spatially and temporarily abstract, and uncertain [e.g., Carvalho, 2010; Duan, Zwickle & Takahashi, 2017; Jones, Hine & Marks, 2017; O’Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009].

Climate futures can be defined as situations, which — from a point of reference — lie in the future, carry an evaluation, and are related to the impacts of climate change [Kosow & Gaßner, 2008; see also Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022]. They can be based on scenarios that are often used in expert/scientific projections (e.g., most famously the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) reports), but also on political ideas (e.g., the Green New Deal), or in some cases even science fiction. There is more to them than just describing “threats of chaotic disruptions to ecological, political and economic systems” [Iossifidis & Garforth, 2022, p. 248]; rather, climate futures project possible, probable, and sometimes even desirable scenarios. However, no matter whether climate futures are doomsday scenarios or desirable outlooks [e.g., Fløttum, Gjesdal, Gjerstad, Koteyko & Salway, 2014; Kumpu, 2013], in line with construal level theory [Trope & Liberman, 2010], their psychological distance may not motivate people to act on negative scenarios or work towards desirable ones [e.g., Duan et al., 2017; Duan, Takahashi & Zwickle, 2021].

In this context, journalistic media are still the main sources of information about climate change for many members of the public [e.g., Guenther, Reif, De Silva-Schmidt & Brüggemann, 2022; Murali, Kuwar & Nagendra, 2021; Newman, Fletcher, Schulz, Andı & Nielsen, 2020], connecting this global issue to the lives of audiences [e.g., Nisbet et al., 2018]. Thus, the way climate futures are represented in journalistic media affects how audiences understand them [e.g., Carmichael et al., 2017; Ruiu, 2021; Schäfer & Painter, 2021], including how psychologically distant they perceive them to be [e.g., Duan et al., 2017]. For instance, (visual) representations of climate change as a distant threat and out of individual control can positively affect feelings of powerlessness [e.g., O’Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009] and negatively affect topic engagement [e.g., Ruiu, 2021]. Based on a lack of research on the content-perspective of distance, this study assesses how distant climate futures are represented in journalistic media, by taking both Global North and Global South countries into account (i.e., Germany, India, South Africa, and the US). This allows for a more complete picture of how (distant) journalistic media in different countries portray a global topic: future scenarios of climate change.

2 Climate change and its psychological distance

To study the psychological distance of climate change, construal level theory has often been applied [e.g., Jones et al., 2017; Scannell & Gifford, 2013]. This theory proposes that if an event (or object, person, place) is perceived as psychologically distant, then individuals develop rather abstract and general mental representations, which lack details, and are described as a higher level of construal [e.g., Trope & Liberman, 2010]. The more concrete an event is perceived to be, the lower the level of construal. Psychological distance concerns the link between events and individuals’ (direct) experiences; usually categorized as a temporal, spatial, social, and hypothetical dimension [e.g., Liberman, Sagristano & Trope, 2002; Trope & Liberman, 2003].

These four dimensions can be seen as a continuum between psychologically proximate and distant. The temporal dimension relates to the distance between a perceiver’s now and the time of an event (e.g., a specific climate future). The spatial dimension relates to the geographical location of a perceiver and the location where an event will happen, which can be near or far away. The social dimension relates to the extent to which the event is familiar to the perceiver [e.g., between the self and individuals/social groups associated with the event; see also Duan et al., 2017]. The hypothetical dimension concerns the probability of an event occurring, with uncertain events seen as more distant.

Studies on this topic that relate to climate change communication are usually framing effect studies, which manipulate the distance of climate change impacts [see also Duan et al., 2017]; in turn, this is then linked to the concept of psychological distance. The (visual) studies mentioned earlier [e.g., O’Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009] can also be considered here. Although researchers assume that messages/visuals with local/proximate content, and thus those that should trigger a low level of construal, are more effective compared to those with rather distant content when it comes to concern about climate change, (personal) relevance, or intentions to engage in actions — and found some support for this [e.g., Jones et al., 2017; Song & Bruning, 2016; Wiest, Raymond & Clawson, 2015] — overall, the evidence is mixed [e.g., Altinay, 2017; Shih & Lin, 2017; Spence & Pidgeon, 2010; van Valkengoed, Steg & Perlaviciute, 2023], or may be dependent on certain frames [e.g., Chu & Yang, 2020], or only be applicable to certain audience segments [e.g., Halperin & Walton, 2018]. A typical example in support of this assumption is Scannell and Gifford [2013], who found that representing local climate change impacts, compared to global ones, increases audience engagement with the issue.

Despite some effort made on this topic in (framing) effect studies, in line with the goal of the present study, there are only a few content analytical approaches relying on (or implying) a construal level perspective. Here, as is often stated, a lack of congruence between content and effect studies is present [e.g., Guenther, Jörges, Mahl & Brüggemann, 2023]. Furthermore, the studies that focus on (aspects of) how distant1 climate change and climate futures are represented indicate mixed findings. Duan et al. [2017], with a focus on US newspaper images, found that climate change is portrayed as relatively concrete and with a high level of specificity. In contrast, O’Neill’s [2013] study found an abstract, distancing visual frame in journalistic images. This is also supported by the fact that often politicians and thus elites are shown visually. Climate change has also often been represented as a global issue [e.g., Rebich-Hespanha et al., 2015], which adds to the perceived level of abstraction. Future expectations about climate change in journalistic media are often undefined [e.g., Hellsten, Porter & Nerlich, 2014]. Some researchers find apocalyptic, global doom scenarios [e.g., Fløttum et al., 2014; Kumpu, 2013], sometimes also called impact, consequences, or Pandora’s Box frames that paint negative outlooks and lack reporting on subsequent actions [e.g., Feldman, Hart & Milosevic, 2017]; hence, they are also seen as showing a distant reporting. In contrast, desirable, sustainable future imaginations, sometimes referred to as opportunity or sustainability frames [e.g., Pan, Opgenhaffen & Van Gorp, 2019], seem to be less distant because they paint more concrete scenarios and ask people to act (including individual behavior).

As emphasized earlier, in this study, we specifically focus on climate futures. Such futures usually include a path description and emphasize elements of a possible future, which is in line with construal level theory as they commonly employ a time frame, have a spatial scope, include (social) actors, and contain hypothetical/plausible elements [Kosow & Gaßner, 2008; see also Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022]. Since there are only a few studies that apply the concept of (psychological) distance to journalistic content on climate futures (or can be interpreted as such), since they predominately focus on Western countries (especially the US), and since they show mixed findings, the first research question (RQ1) of this paper is: How distant do journalistic media across four countries (i.e., Germany, India, South Africa, and the US) report on climate futures?

When answering this question, we propose comparing across countries and across types of scenarios (e.g., ecological, economic), for several reasons.

First of all, taking a comparative perspective on countries in the Global North and Global South is more inclusive, as it acknowledges that media systems and (national) journalistic cultures differ, as do the resources available to dedicate coverage to climate change-related issues [e.g., Comfort, Tandoc & Gruszczynski, 2020; Nguyen & Tran, 2019; Schäfer & Painter, 2021].2 For instance, Hase, Mahl, Schäfer and Keller [2021] found that countries of the Global South show a tendency to report less frequently on climate change than Global North countries; at the same time, they focus more on societal dimensions of the issue, such as challenges and implications for society. For Indian newspapers it was found that climate change is often linked to national contexts and events [e.g., Billett, 2010]. Findings like these could mean that journalists in the Global South report less distantly compared to countries of the Global North. Nevertheless, there are aspects of climate change reporting that seem similar around the globe, such as when it comes to the attention given to climate change or the events that trigger reporting [e.g., Conferences of the Parties (COPs), IPCC reports; e.g., Painter & Schäfer, 2018]. Due to the transnational nature of climate change and its research and policy, there could be strong similarities in reporting on climate futures across countries [e.g., Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022; Wessler, Wozniak, Hofer & Lück, 2016]. However, although countries in the Global South are supposed to be more vulnerable to climate change [e.g., Eckstein, Künzel & Schäfer, 2021], there is a lack of research on them — especially in a comparative perspective [see also Comfort et al., 2020, p. 327; Metag, 2016; Nguyen & Tran, 2019; Schäfer & Painter, 2021; Schäfer & Schlichting, 2014]. Thus, the second research question (RQ2) is: Do media across four countries (i.e., Germany, India, South Africa, and the US) differ regarding how distantly they report on climate futures?

Secondly, taking on a comparative perspective across types of scenarios accounts for the fact that most research that focusses on representations of climate change only takes changes in the ecosystem (e.g., rising temperatures, increase of extreme weather events) into account. Climate futures are, however, not just rooted in science (e.g., IPCC reports with their representative concentration and shared socioeconomic pathways), but also in socio-political (e.g., social (in)equality, migration, or a nation’s political system), economic (e.g., strategies of companies/industry, or a nation’s economy), or even individual ideas (e.g., individual habits or lifestyles) [see also Iossifidis & Garforth, 2022], especially since the Paris Agreement in 2015 which helped broaden the topic [see also Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022]. Such scenarios could rely on different bases of knowledge and provide different evaluations. Accounting for different types of scenarios thus allows for a more thorough assessment of how distantly climate futures are represented journalistically. Consequently, the third research question (RQ3) is: What is the connection between types of scenarios and distant reporting on climate futures?

3 Method

3.1 Sample selection and description

To answer the RQs, the present paper mainly relies on a quantitative content analysis. To achieve an inclusive sample selection, for the Global North, we chose Germany and the US, and for the Global South, we chose India and South Africa. These four countries — representatives of four continents — differ in many aspects [e.g., developed vs. developing countries; for climate risks vulnerability, see Eckstein et al., 2021; for concern about climate change, see Fagan & Huang, 2019], but they seem comparable due to their democratic and media systems, global power, and high emissions [see also Brüggemann & Engesser, 2017]. Germany and the US are two leading Western countries; India and South Africa are part of the BRICS group and thus among the world’s fast-growing economies. For each country, due to their (combined) reach [e.g., Newman et al., 2020; see also Murali et al., 2021] and availability in databases, we chose between eleven and fifteen media outlets per country: including print quality newspapers, print regional newspapers, tabloid newspapers, weekly newspapers/magazines, and online newspapers (see Table 1, for an overview). An effort was made to select leading outlets [see also Hase et al., 2021] based on their reach, including different ideological leanings [see also Duan et al., 2017] where applicable. For the regional newspapers, geographic spread was considered (i.e., east, north, west, and south regions of the respective countries). The German media comprise German-language outlets (as this is the official language of the country), the other countries comprise English-language outlets. It should be mentioned that in each of the other countries, the English language and English-language media have a prominent place, although there are media available in other languages (e.g., Spanish, Hindi, Afrikaans, Zulu). Most importantly this is the case for India and South Africa. However, the general availability of English-language media was part of why these countries were chosen in the first place.

To download relevant content, i.e., coverage on climate change, validated search strings were used, which were based on literature reviews and search term mining [for detailed information, see Mahl, von Nordheim & Guenther, 2023]: the search strings were “atleast2 climat* change AND (climat* change OR global warm* OR greenhouse effect OR greenhouse gas*)” (precision = .80; recall = .80; F1 = .80) for English and “Klimawandel* OR globale Erwärmung OR Treibhauseffekt* OR Erderwärmung OR Klimakrise” (precision = .79; recall = .97; F1 = .87) for German outlets. Since we worked with two languages, two different search strings had to be used: a simple translation from one language into the other did not yield appropriate values for precision and recall. Hence, the two search strings are not equivalent, but the same method to generate and validate them was applied [see Mahl et al., 2023].

Due to the large number of media outlets included in this study, we relied on several databases (i.e., Factiva, LexisNexis, FAZ Bibliotheksportal, Sabinet SA Media, and the Online Media Monitor) and considered January 2017–December 2020 (i.e., four years) a relevant time frame in which climate change gained high visibility in the media (for instance, due to the aftermath of the Paris Agreement and global climate protests). After checking for duplicates, the sample contained 56,394 articles. The articles were not spread equally across countries and media outlets (see Table 1). This finding was to be expected, since tabloid newspapers or media in countries such as South Africa report less often on climate change [see also Hase et al., 2021].

PIC
Table 1. Sample and study sample description.

3.2 Content analytical approaches

This article reports on research that was conducted as part of a larger project; here we focus mainly on findings of our manual content analysis. A dictionary-derivation approach was applied to identify articles that likely contained a climate change-related future scenario [for more information, see Guenther, Meyer, Kleinen-von Königslöw & Brüggemann, 2023]: we started with qualitative coding and extraction of climate future text passages of a random sample of 700 articles (across countries). The most common words (1,000 for English and 2,000 for German articles) were assessed and evaluated by two individuals involved in the research project. Work of colleagues [e.g., on journalists’ use of temporal references or reporting on climate change; Baden & Stalpouskaya, 2015; Wozniak, Wessler, Chan & Lück, 2021] and translations of words between the two languages included were considered when developing the dictionaries. The two dictionaries (191 English and 210 German (combinations of) words) were tested, evaluated, validated, and normalized; they assigned a relevance-score to each article, based on the matches regarding each dictionary term (which were not weighted; each occurrence was counted once per article; for more information, see Guenther, Meyer et al. [2023]). Hence, the dictionaries determined the ranking of articles per country. With the goal to manually code 1,000 articles, at the end, in total, n = 1, 010 articles were coded.

The codebook used for the actual analysis contained formal (e.g., type of article, broad topic) and content-related categories. The latter assessed climate future scenario(s) presented in the articles: either climate/ecosystem (e.g., rising temperatures, increase of extreme weather events), economic system (e.g., companies, industry (sectors), economy of a nation (in general), socio-political system (e.g., social (in)equality, migration, or political system of a nation in general), or individual scenarios (e.g., individual habits or lifestyles). For each identified scenario, evaluations (e.g., none, negative, ambivalent, or positive) and categories with a reference to (psychological) distance [see also Duan et al., 2017]3 were integrated:

  • the time frame (i.e., unclear or on a continuum between near (this or next year) and distant (50+ years), related to temporal distance),

  • the scope (i.e., unclear or on a continuum between near (regional/local) and distant (global), related to spatial distance),

  • the actors associated with this scenario (e.g., for all actors present: distant/elite actors such as scientists or political actors and close actors such as citizens/individuals, related to social distance),

  • and the plausibility (i.e., unclear or on a continuum between very unlikely to very likely, related to hypothetical distance).

We allowed for more than one scenario to be present in an article; hence, all evaluations and categories with a reference to (psychological) distance could be coded several times in an article. The codebook used in this study is available online in Guenther, Meyer et al. [2023].

Three coders were thoroughly trained in ten training sessions over four months, to use the codebook. During that time, the codebook was adjusted to increase understanding and assure that all coders used it the same way. A number of articles were coded in these sessions, first together, then independently, with exhaustive comparisons and discussions. Intercoder reliability was assessed after 45 articles were coded in additional sessions, with two random samples of 15 and 30 articles, respectively. Using Krippendorff’s Alpha (and Holsti, as a check), the coders reached satisfactory results, with the average scores for the formal (α = .93; CR = .97) and the content-related categories (scenarios and categories: α = .85; CR = .90; actors: α = .83; CR = .93) in an acceptable range. The authors are aware that they detached from the 10% criterion for testing intercoder reliability; however, they still assured that the training was complex, thorough, and successful. After intercoder reliability was established, there were regular check-ins, to discuss progress and problems.

4 Results

Most of the articles in the sample were published in 2019 (n = 329; 33%), fewer were published in 2018 (n = 252; 25%), 2017 (n = 217; 22%), and 2020 (n = 212; 21%). As Table 1 indicates, the largest share of articles was from online newspapers (n = 544; 54%), with print quality newspapers (n = 261; 26%) and print regional newspapers (n = 171; 17%) as second and third. Furthermore, most articles in the sample were coded as an original journalistic article (n = 891; 88%), having consequences of climate change (n = 687; 68%), mitigation (n = 193; 19%), or adaptation (n = 80; 8%) as a broad topic. In total, in the 1,010 articles, 1,262 future scenarios were reported on.

Regarding RQ1 (see also Table 2), climate/ecosystem scenarios such as rising temperatures, sea level rise, an increasing number of extreme weather events, or habitat loss of plant and animal species were most dominant (in all countries), followed by economic scenarios such as those referring to individual companies, industry sectors (e.g., agriculture, tourism), or a nation’s economy in general and socio-political scenarios (e.g., social (in)equality, migration (climate refugees), supply of drinking water and food). There were only a few individual scenarios; hence, scenarios that address individual habits or lifestyle. Most scenarios were negatively evaluated. When it came to the time frame, it was usually unclear or far in the future (i.e., more than 50 years) — indicating a high level of distance. Regarding the geographic scope, this was most commonly global or distant, again indicating a high level of distance. For social distance, many frequent actors were rather distant (i.e., scientific or political actors) while some were less distant (i.e., citizens/individuals). Lastly, the media in the sample predominantly assessed climate futures’ plausibility as (very) likely, showing the lowest level of distance across the four dimensions.

PIC
Table 2. Type of future scenarios and categories of distance across the sample.

Regarding RQ2, there was little variation across countries. In all countries, climate/ecosystem scenarios were most common; economic and socio-political scenarios were slightly more frequent in Indian and South African, compared to US and German, media (for values and statistical tests, see Table 2). The tendency of most scenarios to be negatively evaluated was less dominant in South African media. The countries only slightly varied regarding the time frame, but there was a weak tendency of German and US media to report with more temporal distance than those in India and South Africa (for a visualization of categories related to distance, see Figure 1). There was more variation regarding the geographic scope, which more so in German media was most commonly global or distant. US media seemed to include citizens/individuals more often than the other countries. At the same time, however, US media — as with the other countries — also most frequently included the perspectives of scientific actors, which are seen as rather socially distant. Hence, Figure 1 shows that there are not many differences between countries overall. Nonetheless, across all countries, media assessed climate futures’ plausibility as (very) likely, but slightly more so in Indian and South African media.

PIC

Figure 1. Distance across countries. Notes. The outer the layer, the more distance is present. Categories recoded into 4-points (excluding “unclear” coding; time frame: current–5 years, more than 5–30 years, more than 30–50 years, more than 50 years; geographic scope: local/national, continental, other part of the world, global; social: number of social distant actors (e.g., scientists, politicians) minus number of social close actors (e.g., citizens) — and recoded; plausibility: very unlikely, somewhat/rather unlikely, likely, very likely/certain).

With reference to RQ3, while there was little variation across countries, how distant media reported on climate futures seemed to be slightly more dependent on the type of scenario (see Table 3 and Figure 2 for the categories related to distance). For the climate/ecosystem scenarios, which were mostly negative scenarios, the time frame was often either unclear or far in the future (50+ years), the scope was often on other parts of the world or was global, and the plausibility was most frequently likely (i.e., use of the conjunctive). The main actors associated were scientists. In total, climate/ecosystem scenarios were thus the most distant (and thus in Figure 2, they represent the outer circle). The economic scenarios were not as distant: their evaluation was more mixed, with negative ones most common but ambivalent and positive evaluations were also present. The time frame was often unclear or concerned the next 5–30 years, the scope was frequently local or national (while still most often global), and the plausibility assessed as likely and, more often, very likely. Hence, all these categories showed, at least to some degree, a slightly lower level of distance. The most dominant actors associated with these scenarios were nevertheless distant: local/national or international political actors, scientists, and economic actors.

PIC
Table 3. Categories of distance across types of future scenarios.

PIC

Figure 2. Distance across types of scenarios. Notes. The outer the layer, the more distance is present. Categories recoded into 4-points (excluding “unclear” coding; time frame: current–5 years, more than 5–30 years, more than 30–50 years, more than 50 years; geographic scope: local/national, continental, other part of the world, global; social: number of social distant actors (e.g., scientists, politicians) minus number of social close actors (e.g., citizens) — and recoded; plausibility: very unlikely, somewhat/rather unlikely, likely, very likely/certain).

Similarly, socio-political scenarios were not as distant as climate/ecosystem scenarios. Although they had the highest frequency of negative evaluations and unclear time frames, often a focus on other parts of the world or a global scope, and were assessed as rather likely, these scenarios more often than before included citizens/individuals as actors. However, the least distant, but at the same time least frequently reported on, scenarios were the individual ones, which also showed mixed evaluations. They represented many different time frames, often with a local or national scope, mostly represented as very likely. These scenarios are the ones that most frequently linked to citizens/individuals and are thus not seen as distant as the other three types of scenarios.

5 Discussion

Although this varies across the globe [e.g., Fagan & Huang, 2019], climate change is (still) not a major cause for concern for many members of the public [e.g., Bell et al., 2021; Carmichael et al., 2017] — with psychological distance among the potential reasons for that. Since journalistic media are influential sources about the topic, this article analyzed how distant media across four countries reported on climate futures.

This study found that in journalistic media across the Global North and Global South countries under investigation in this study, there was only little variation in how distantly they reported on the topic. Ecosystem scenarios dominated in all countries, but slightly more so in the Global North countries Germany and the US. Indian and South African media, hence those from the Global South, showed a slight tendency to more frequently cover economic and socio-political scenarios — probably also the reason why, in total, these two countries from the Global South had less distant reporting regarding time frames, scopes, and plausibility. Altogether, Global South countries show at least a tendency to exhibit less distant reporting on climate futures [see also Hase et al., 2021, for their findings on societal dimensions in Global South reporting]; however, the lack of more country-specific differences may also be related to the transnational character of climate change and climate policy [e.g., Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022; Wessler et al., 2016]. At the same time, the lack of cross-country differences may also be related to the codebook used in the present study, which may have used categories too broad to detect country differences. In total, nevertheless, climate futures portrayed in journalistic media seem to be presented rather distant, which could potentially complicate linking them to daily life experiences of members of the audience.

Comparing the countries, it seemed that the type of climate future scenario was connected to how distantly they were represented journalistically. Thus, not all climate change-related future scenarios are equally distant. References to the climate/ecosystem are the most distant. Due to their scientific base, they often refer to the years 2050 or 2100; due to their high frequency, they add to the perception that climate futures are removed from the daily life experiences of individuals [for similar explanations, see Carvalho, 2010; Duan et al., 2017; O’Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009]. Such a reporting is possibly not beneficial for topic engagement [e.g., Ruiu, 2021], as it probably leads to abstract and general mental representations in individuals [e.g., Trope & Liberman, 2003, 2010]; however, these scenarios seem to follow the logic of scientific assessment most closely. Climate research rather draws scenarios for years such as 2050 or 2100, but less so for the immediate future to come.

A more concrete reporting was identified for both economic (e.g., closer scope and time frame) and socio-political (e.g., more certainty) climate futures; at the same time, they are not as frequently reported on as the climate/ecosystem scenarios. Individual scenarios are the least distant, but they are almost absent from coverage. Such scenarios, also due to their focus on individuals, could probably motivate people to act [e.g., Jones et al., 2017; Scannell & Gifford, 2013; Wiest et al., 2015] because they more strongly relate to everyday life. Hence, if journalists wanted to contribute to making climate change more of a priority to their audiences and make them act on negative scenarios or work towards desirable ones, reporting on climate futures could be altered along the temporal, spatial, and social dimensions of (psychological) distance — independent of the type of scenario represented. The hypothetical dimension is the only one that showed a low level of distance in this sample: climate futures are represented as (rather) certain. Naturally, journalists are dependent on their sources and information provided; nevertheless, they could hold those in powerful positions accountable and ask them to state, comment, or judge on the immediate climate future.

The findings of this study are to some degree in line with some earlier studies on (visual) representations of climate change and climate futures [e.g., Fløttum et al., 2014; Guenther, Brüggemann & Elkobros, 2022; Hellsten et al., 2014; Kumpu, 2013; O’Neill, 2013; Rebich-Hespanha et al., 2015], but to some degree in contrast to a study applying construal level theory to visuals in newspapers [e.g., Duan et al., 2017]. However, in the study by Duan et al. [2017], many categories assessed if information was given; in the present study, we assessed if this information can be seen as rather proximate or distant, on a continuum. Thus, we believe that the present study can be seen as an extension of the coding of textual content. We also added a comparison across countries.

Nevertheless, the present study also has some notable limitations. An effort was made to include several different media in countries of both the Global North and Global South; however, selecting only four countries, two languages, and focusing on print and online journalism is a limitation. Certainly, relying on human coders meant that only a small number of articles from a bigger sample could be analyzed in detail. Furthermore, including a variety of different journalistic media and using several databases meant the study had to rely on a rather small time frame. Not all media sources are represented equally (cf. Table 1) and certainly the low number of articles in tabloid media and weekly outlets is a further limitation. In addition, some operationalizations are still up for debate; for instance, who counts as a distant compared to a close actor (e.g., local and national political actors).

Future research could link the findings of this study back to audiences and test if real journalistic content coded as distant (as compared to manipulated stimuli) is indeed leading members of the audience to assess climate change as an abstract topic on which they show limited intentions to act, as well as if representing information as less distant can counter this effect. A recent review [van Valkengoed et al., 2023] shows that distance may not be the key variable to explain climate (in)action; nevertheless, journalistic media still portray climate futures as not here, not now, not me, although extreme weather events such as draughts and heat waves already regularly affect people around the globe.

6 Funding

This work was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy — EXC 2037 “CLICCS — Climate, Climatic Change, and Society” — Project Number: 390683824, contribution to the Center for Earth System Research and Sustainability (CEN) of Universität Hamburg.

References

Altinay, Z. (2017). Visual communication of climate change: local framing and place attachment. Coastal Management 45 (4), 293–309. doi:10.1080/08920753.2017.1327344

Baden, C. & Stalpouskaya, K. (2015). Common methodological framework: content analysis. A mixed-methods strategy for comparatively, diachronically analyzing conflict discourse [INFOCORE Working Paper 2015/10]. Retrieved from http://www.infocore.eu/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/Methodological-Paper-MWG-CA_final.pdf

Bell, J., Poushter, J., Fagan, M. & Huang, C. (2021). In response to climate change, citizens in advanced economies are willing to alter how they live and work. Pew Research Center. Washington, DC, U.S.A. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2021/09/14/in-response-to-climate-change-citizens-in-advanced-economies-are-willing-to-alter-how-they-live-and-work/

Billett, S. (2010). Dividing climate change: global warming in the Indian mass media. Climatic Change 99 (1–2), 1–16. doi:10.1007/s10584-009-9605-3

Brüggemann, M. & Engesser, S. (2017). Beyond false balance: how interpretive journalism shapes media coverage of climate change. Global Environmental Change 42, 58–67. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.11.004

Carmichael, J. T., Brulle, R. J. & Huxster, J. K. (2017). The great divide: understanding the role of media and other drivers of the partisan divide in public concern over climate change in the USA, 2001–2014. Climatic Change 141 (4), 599–612. doi:10.1007/s10584-017-1908-1

Carvalho, A. (2010). Media(ted) discourses and climate change: a focus on political subjectivity and (dis)engagement. WIREs Climate Change 1 (2), 172–179. doi:10.1002/wcc.13

Chu, H. & Yang, J. Z. (2020). Risk or efficacy? How psychological distance influences climate change engagement. Risk Analysis 40 (4), 758–770. doi:10.1111/risa.13446

Comfort, S. E., Tandoc, E. & Gruszczynski, M. (2020). Who is heard in climate change journalism? Sourcing patterns in climate change news in China, India, Singapore, and Thailand. Climatic Change 158 (3–4), 327–343. doi:10.1007/s10584-019-02597-1

Duan, R., Takahashi, B. & Zwickle, A. (2021). How effective are concrete and abstract climate change images? The moderating role of construal level in climate change visual communication. Science Communication 43 (3), 358–387. doi:10.1177/10755470211008192

Duan, R., Zwickle, A. & Takahashi, B. (2017). A construal-level perspective of climate change images in US newspapers. Climatic Change 142 (3–4), 345–360. doi:10.1007/s10584-017-1945-9

Eckstein, D., Künzel, V. & Schäfer, L. (2021). Global climate risk index. Germanwatch. Retrieved from https://www.germanwatch.org/en/19777

Fagan, M. & Huang, C. (2019). A look at how people around the world view climate change. Pew Research Center. Washington, DC, U.S.A. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/04/18/a-look-at-how-people-around-the-world-view-climate-change/

Feldman, L., Hart, P. S. & Milosevic, T. (2017). Polarizing news? Representations of threat and efficacy in leading US newspapers’ coverage of climate change. Public Understanding of Science 26 (4), 481–497. doi:10.1177/0963662515595348

Fløttum, K., Gjesdal, A. M., Gjerstad, Ø., Koteyko, N. & Salway, A. (2014). Representations of the future in English language blogs on climate change. Global Environmental Change 29, 213–222. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.10.005

Guenther, L., Brüggemann, M. & Elkobros, S. (2022). From Global Doom to Sustainable Solutions: international news magazines’ multimodal framing of our future with climate change. Journalism Studies 23 (1), 131–148. doi:10.1080/1461670x.2021.2007162

Guenther, L., Jörges, S., Mahl, D. & Brüggemann, M. (2023). Framing as a bridging concept for climate change communication: a systematic review based on 25 years of literature. Communication Research. doi:10.1177/00936502221137165

Guenther, L., Meyer, H., Kleinen-von Königslöw, K. & Brüggemann, M. (2023). A distant threat? The framing of climate futures across four countries. Environmental Communication. doi:10.1080/17524032.2023.2253500

Guenther, L., Reif, A., De Silva-Schmidt, F. & Brüggemann, M. (2022). Klimawandel und Klimapolitik bleiben trotz COVID-19-Pandemie etablierte Themen. Media Perspektiven 4, 192–202. Retrieved from https://www.ard-media.de/media-perspektiven/publikationsarchiv/2022/detailseite-2022/klimawandel-und-klimapolitik-bleiben-trotz-covid-19-pandemie-etablierte-themen

Halperin, A. & Walton, P. (2018). The importance of place in communicating climate change to different facets of the American public. Weather, Climate, and Society 10 (2), 291–305. doi:10.1175/WCAS-D-16-0119.1

Hase, V., Mahl, D., Schäfer, M. S. & Keller, T. R. (2021). Climate change in news media across the globe: an automated analysis of issue attention and themes in climate change coverage in 10 countries (2006–2018). Global Environmental Change 70, 102353. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2021.102353

Hellsten, I., Porter, A. J. & Nerlich, B. (2014). Imagining the future at the global and national scale: a comparative study of British and Dutch press coverage of Rio 1992 and Rio 2012. Environmental Communication 8 (4), 468–488. doi:10.1080/17524032.2014.911197

Iossifidis, M. J. M. & Garforth, L. (2022). Reimagining climate futures: reading Annihilation. Geoforum 137, 248–257. doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2021.12.001

Jones, C., Hine, D. W. & Marks, A. D. G. (2017). The future is now: reducing psychological distance to increase public engagement with climate change. Risk Analysis 37 (2), 331–341. doi:10.1111/risa.12601

Kosow, H. & Gaßner, R. (2008). Methods of future and scenario analysis. Overview, assessment, and selection criteria. Deutsches Institut für Entwicklungspolitik. Bonn, Germany. Retrieved from https://www.idos-research.de/uploads/media/Studies_39.2008.pdf

Kumpu, V. (2013). A climate for reduction? Futures imagined in newspaper coverage of UN climate summits. Futures 53, 53–62. doi:10.1016/j.futures.2013.08.007

Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D. & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38 (6), 523–534. doi:10.1016/s0022-1031(02)00535-8

Mahl, D., von Nordheim, G. & Guenther, L. (2023). Noise pollution: a multi-step approach to assessing the consequences of (not) validating search terms on automated content analyses. Digital Journalism 11 (2), 298–320. doi:10.1080/21670811.2022.2114920

Metag, J. (2016). Content analysis in climate change communication. In Oxford research encyclopedia of climate science. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.486

Murali, R., Kuwar, A. & Nagendra, H. (2021). Who’s responsible for climate change? Untangling threads of media discussions in India, Nigeria, Australia, and the USA. Climatic Change 164 (3–4), 51. doi:10.1007/s10584-021-03031-1

Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Schulz, A., Andı, S. & Nielsen, R. K. (2020). Reuters Institute digital news report 2020. Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism, University of Oxford. Retrieved from https://www.digitalnewsreport.org/survey/2020/

Nguyen, A. & Tran, M. (2019). Science journalism for development in the Global South: a systematic literature review of issues and challenges. Public Understanding of Science 28 (8), 973–990. doi:10.1177/0963662519875447

Nisbet, M. C., Ho, S. S., Markowitz, E., O’Neill, S., Schäfer, M. S. & Thaker, J. (2018). The Oxford encyclopedia of climate change communication. Oxford University Press. Retrieved from https://oxfordre.com/climatescience/page/climate-change-communication/the-oxford-encyclopedia-of-climatechange-communication

O’Neill, S. (2013). Image matters: climate change imagery in US, UK and Australian newspapers. Geoforum 49, 10–19. doi:10.1016/j.geoforum.2013.04.030

O’Neill, S. & Nicholson-Cole, S. (2009). “Fear won’t do it”: promoting positive engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. Science Communication 30 (3), 355–379. doi:10.1177/1075547008329201

Painter, J. & Schäfer, M. S. (2018). Global similarities and persistent differences: a survey of comparative studies on climate change communication. In B. Brevini & J. Lewis (Eds.), Climate change and the media (pp. 122–136). doi:10.3726/b14826

Pan, Y., Opgenhaffen, M. & Van Gorp, B. (2019). Negotiating climate change: a frame analysis of COP21 in British, American, and Chinese news media. Public Understanding of Science 28 (5), 519–533. doi:10.1177/0963662518823969

Rebich-Hespanha, S., Rice, R. E., Montello, D. R., Retzloff, S., Tien, S. & Hespanha, J. P. (2015). Image themes and frames in US print news stories about climate change. Environmental Communication 9 (4), 491–519. doi:10.1080/17524032.2014.983534

Ruiu, M. L. (2021). Representation of climate change consequences in British newspapers. European Journal of Communication 36 (5), 478–493. doi:10.1177/0267323120978727

Scannell, L. & Gifford, R. (2013). Personally relevant climate change: the role of place attachment and local versus global message framing in engagement. Environment and Behavior 45 (1), 60–85. doi:10.1177/0013916511421196

Schäfer, M. S. & Painter, J. (2021). Climate journalism in a changing media ecosystem: assessing the production of climate change-related news around the world. WIREs Climate Change 12 (1), e675. doi:10.1002/wcc.675

Schäfer, M. S. & Schlichting, I. (2014). Media representations of climate change: a meta-analysis of the research field. Environmental Communication 8 (2), 142–160. doi:10.1080/17524032.2014.914050

Shih, T.-J. & Lin, C.-Y. (2017). Developing communication strategies for mitigating actions against global warming: linking framing and a dual processing model. Environmental Communication 11 (6), 840–858. doi:10.1080/17524032.2016.1154886

Song, M. & Bruning, R. (2016). Exploring effects of background context familiarity and signaling on comprehension, recall, and cognitive load. Educational Psychology 36 (4), 691–718. doi:10.1080/01443410.2015.1072133

Spence, A. & Pidgeon, N. (2010). Framing and communicating climate change: the effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change 20 (4), 656–667. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.07.002

Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review 110 (3), 403–421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403

Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review 117 (2), 440–463. doi:10.1037/a0018963

United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change (2008). Kyoto Protocol reference manual. On accounting of emissions and assigned amount. Retrieved from https://unfccc.int/sites/default/files/08_unfccc_kp_ref_manual.pdf

van Valkengoed, A. M., Steg, L. & Perlaviciute, G. (2023). The psychological distance of climate change is overestimated. One Earth 6 (4), 362–391. doi:10.1016/j.oneear.2023.03.006

Wessler, H., Wozniak, A., Hofer, L. & Lück, J. (2016). Global multimodal news frames on climate change: a comparison of five democracies around the world. The International Journal of Press/Politics 21 (4), 423–445. doi:10.1177/1940161216661848

Wiest, S. L., Raymond, L. & Clawson, R. A. (2015). Framing, partisan predispositions, and public opinion on climate change. Global Environmental Change 31, 187–198. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.12.006

Wozniak, A., Wessler, H., Chan, C.-h. & Lück, J. (2021). The event-centered nature of global public spheres: the UN Climate Change Conferences, Fridays for Future, and the (limited) transnationalization of media debates. International Journal of Communication 15, 688–714. Retrieved from https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/14843

Authors

Lars Guenther (Ph.D., 2015, at Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Germany) is Professor of Communication Science at LMU Munich’s Department of Media and Communication in Germany, and Extraordinary Associate Professor at the Centre for Research on Evaluation, Science and Technology (CREST) at Stellenbosch University in South Africa. He is interested into public perceptions of (controversial) science, science and health journalism, trust in science, as well as the public communication about risks and scientific (un)certainty.
E-mail: lars.guenther@ifkw.lmu.de

Michael Brüggemann (Ph.D., 2007, at University of Hamburg) is Professor for Communication Studies, Climate and Science Communication at the Institute for Journalism and Communication Studies, University of Hamburg. As a principal investigator in the clusters of excellence CliSAP (until 2018) and CLICCS (since 2019), his research focuses on climate and science communication and its effects on society.
@MichaelBruegge E-mail: michael.brueggemann@uni-hamburg.de

Endnotes

1Since the concept of ‘psychological distance’ describes a subjective concept that is not suitable when describing media content, we rather refer to ‘distance’ in this context.

2Besides media contexts, social contexts (e.g., political and economic systems, number of people denying climate change) need to also be considered in country comparisons.

3In contrast to many of the categories in Duan et al. [2017], who focused on visuals, we did not just assess if information about these dimensions was given but also to what degree it can be assessed on a continuum between close or distant.