1 Introduction

Expert knowledge is neither generated nor received by its intended audience in a neutral, value-free context [Connor and Siegrist, 2010 ; Mogendorff, te Molder, van Woerkum et al., 2016 ; Yearley, 2000 ]. When scientific knowledge is used in communication strategies around risk situations such as disease outbreaks, the underlying purpose driving such communication is usually compliance and reduction of uncertainty [Brashers, 2001 ; Davis, Stephenson and Flowers, 2011 ]. This influences both the message and its distribution, and dictates the measurement of successful communication [Davis, Stephenson and Flowers, 2011 ; Fischhoff, Slovic and Lichtenstein, 1982 ]. If a compliant response to a communication strategy is considered the only ‘correct’ response, the nuances of actual responses may be misunderstood or ignored [Davis, Stephenson, Lohm et al., 2015 ]. Likewise, if the default communication paradigm is uncertainty reduction, then opportunities to manage the complexity of uncertainty will be limited [Brashers, 2001 ]. This can result in frustration for stakeholders, especially for those with training in risk decision making and decision makers considered at risk [Garvin, 2001 ; Kung et al., 2013 ; Manyweathers, Field, Longnecker et al., 2017 ; Slovic, 1999 ].

The selection of what constitutes legitimate responses to communication strategies is underpinned by institutional cultures that surrounds scientific research, particularly involving expertise [Mogendorff, te Molder, Gremmen et al., 2012 ; Wynne, 1998 ] and management of uncertainty [Brashers, 2001 ; Han, 2016 ; Priest, 2006 ]. Institutional cultures inform an often prescriptive approach to risk communication that disqualifies reflection about how local knowledge and uncertainty may impact responses to risk and decision making [Brashers, Goldsmith and Hsieh, 2002 ; Horst, 2008 ; Metcalfe, 2019 ; Wynne, 1998 ]. This lack of reflection may result in decreased trust between stakeholders and hinder participatory communication around risk and response to disease outbreaks.

Risk communication strategies addressing newly emerging diseases are complex. Risk mitigation strategies are not initially available and the scope of uncertainty is vast for all involved [Bangerter, Green and Gilles, 2011 ; Christley et al., 2013 ; Holland et al., 2012 ]. Nor is expertise the sole prerequisite on which risk communication messengers can claim credibility [Fiske and Dupree, 2014 ]. With communicator credibility found to be a shortcut for audience acceptance of scientific messages [Brewer and Ley, 2013 ], understanding the role of trust in establishing messenger credibility is important for effective risk management strategies and the control of disease outbreaks.

When risk communication involves diseases in domestic animals, the human-animal bond creates an added complexity to the communication and management of outbreaks [Anderson, 1997 ; Enticott and Vanclay, 2011 ]. If this bond is underestimated, or owner expertise is not considered legitimate, serious negative consequences can arise [Convery et al., 2005 ; Franklin, 2001 ; Rogers, 2016 ; Taylor, Lynch et al., 2015 ]. Without consultation and collaboration, disease management protocols may be misunderstood, mocked or ignored [Bangerter, Green and Gilles, 2011 ; Hernández-Jover, Taylor et al., 2012 ; Jensen, 2004 ; Manyweathers, Field, Longnecker et al., 2017 ].

Communication around zoonoses (diseases that spread from animals to humans) are even more complex because of the added risk for owners and veterinarians [Jensen, 2004 ; Ochieng’ and Hobbs, 2016 ; Triezenberg et al., 2014 ]. Veterinarians are often identified as trusted sources of information around biosecurity generally [Brennan and Christley, 2013 ; Hernández-Jover, Higgins et al., 2016 ]. Yet there are many factors that influence whether people engage with new information and whether they act on it [Longnecker, 2016 ] and veterinarians, like many experts, may not have a deep understanding of science communication. When both humans and animals are at risk of disease, veterinarians can be required to undertake a significant public health role for which they may not be well rehearsed, while also needing to protect themselves [Mendez, Judd and Speare, 2012 ]. This new role, with its required communication skills, needs to be better understood to facilitate preparation by the profession for future zoonotic disease outbreaks [Dowd et al., 2013 ; Manyweathers, Field, Jordan et al., 2017 ]. The lessons learned are valuable for various experts in different contexts of risk communication.

1.1 Hendra virus and the development of a vaccine for horses

Hendra virus is a zoonosis that has caused fatal infections in both horses and humans, with humans being infected from direct contact with sick horses. [Field, 2016 ; Mahalingam et al., 2012 ]. The natural hosts, characterised by subclinical infections, are pteropid bats (flying-foxes) [Edson et al., 2015 ; Field et al., 2015 ; Halpin et al., 2000 ]. To date, there have been seven humans cases, of which four (57%) have been fatal [Goldspink et al., 2015 ]. Of these human cases, five have been veterinarians, or para-veterinary staff.

A vaccine against Hendra virus for use in horses, and consequently providing protection for humans, was developed in 2011 after an unprecedented number of 23 horse cases, over a wide geographic area [Broder, Weir and Reid, 2016 ; Middleton et al., 2014 ]. The vaccine was released under restricted permit use in November 2012. This initial release was pending full registration with the Australian Pesticides and Veterinary Medicines Association (APVMA), the Australian Government body responsible for the registration of agricultural and veterinary chemicals. The vaccine could be used under strict conditions including administration by an accredited veterinarian, and the microchipping of inoculated horses. Since 2012, the permit for the Hendra virus vaccine has undergone several iterations, including instructions for six-monthly boosters. In August 2015, the vaccine became fully registered, with 12 monthly boosters being recommended from May 2016. To date, no vaccinated horses have been diagnosed with Hendra virus, despite periodic cases in unvaccinated horses.

The state government agencies responsible for Hendra virus policy are the Queensland Department for Agriculture and Fisheries (QDAF) and New South Wales Department of Primary Industries (NSW DPI). They recommend vaccination as ‘the single most effective way of reducing the risk of Hendra virus infection in horses ’ [Queensland Government, 2013 ]. However, horse owners have been hesitant in their uptake of the vaccine [Middleton et al., 2014 ]. Whilst there is no national registry of horses in Australia, it is estimated that the majority of horses remain unvaccinated, with rates as low as 11% in New South Wales and 17% in Queensland [Taylor, Dhand et al., 2016 ].

1.2 Communication challenges

During the first ever Australian outbreak of Equine influenza in 2007, horse owners identified information coming from veterinarians as significantly more efficacious than information from other sources [Schemann et al., 2011 ]. Veterinarians are still identified as one of the most preferred and used sources of information around Hendra virus [Hii et al., 2020 ]. However, since the vaccine release, discourse among some horse owners and veterinarians around Hendra virus and the vaccine has become divisive. Divisiveness has been exacerbated by several factors, including the exclusion of unvaccinated horses from veterinary care by some veterinary practices and prosecution by Work Health and Safety officers of veterinarians for failure of duty of care around management of suspected Hendra virus cases. Equine veterinarians have left the profession as a result [Mendez, Judd and Speare, 2012 ] and a state government enquiry around the handling of the vaccine has added to discourse complexity [Queensland Government, 2016 ].

A project was launched to facilitate a deeper understanding of the factors involved in uptake of Hendra virus risk mitigation strategies. The Hendra Virus Infection and Transmission Dynamics — Conversation with Industry project was commissioned in Australia by an intergovernmental Hendra Virus Taskforce [Australian Government, 2016 ; Taylor, Dhand et al., 2016 ]. Overall, the project was directed to assess the level of uptake of government-recommended Hendra virus risk mitigation practices and to investigate the barriers and enablers that influence this uptake.

This study builds on findings from that project. Designed in two stages, the first part of this study involved an online survey targeted towards horse owners living in areas near previous Hendra virus cases, who had elected not to vaccinate any or all of their horses. The survey was designed to examine risk mitigation practices, risk perception, and horse owners’ attitudes towards the risk of Hendra virus, with results being reported elsewhere [Manyweathers, Field, Jordan et al., 2017 ; Manyweathers, Field, Longnecker et al., 2017 ]. This paper focuses on the second stage of the study, using in-depth interviews to explore the nature of engagement around Hendra virus among veterinarians and horse owners who had elected not to vaccinate their horses. The aim of the study was to explore the perspectives of those considered to be non-compliant with a recommended practice and the experts tasked with providing both information and the vaccinations. The results can be used to inform future risk communication strategies and management of infectious diseases, both emerging and re-emerging.

2 Methodology

In-depth interviews were conducted with a subsample of survey respondents (horse owners who had elected not to vaccinate some or all of their horses) and practising veterinarians in areas known to be at risk of Hendra virus. Table 1 provides information regarding interview participants. The majority of respondents identified as female. This apparent gender bias is in fact representative of the Australian horse owning population [Hii et al., 2020 ; Taylor, Dhand et al., 2016 ].


Table 1 : Interviewee characteristics.
PIC

Overall 25 in depth interviews lasting 60 to 120 minutes were carried out by the first author: 23 face to face and two by phone. There were 10 interviews with veterinarians, including four interviews with equine veterinarians and pathologists from the James Cook University veterinary school in Townsville, Queensland. Interviews were conducted with 15 horse owners. Interviews were carried out in 2015, one month before the Hendra virus vaccine for horses was fully registered.

Non-vaccinating horse owners were identified from an online survey carried out between January and March 2015. All respondent data from the survey were collected from SurveyMonkey TM into Excel TM files. The only respondents considered for interviews were those who had completed the survey, consented to further contact, included their contact details and were located near areas of previous Hendra virus cases.

In order to ensure good coverage of locations, a small interview sample size for each location was required. The strength of this methodology lies with the ‘thick description’ that comes with the semi-structured nature of the data collection [Holliday, 2002 , p. 77]. However, the sample may not represent all horse owners in each area visited. The representativeness of this sample can be considered in the light of other Hendra virus studies that suggests that these experiences and opinions are consistent with those reported by the horse owning community in areas where Hendra virus cases have been reported [Hii et al., 2020 ; Mendez, Büttner et al., 2016 ; Wiethoelter et al., 2017 ].

2.1 Interview protocol

Different interview questions were developed, after multiple iterations, for horse owners and veterinarians [Manyweathers, 2017 ]. Interviews were semi-structured, adopting a conversational approach with questions used as prompts [Brinkmann and Kvale, 2015 ]. The interview protocols for both horse owners and veterinarians are provided as supplementary material.

Ethics approval for the study was provided by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Western Sydney (Protocol No. H10643).

2.2 Data Analysis

Each interview was digitally recorded, transcribed and downloaded into NVivo TM . Comments from individual horse owners were identified with an ‘O’ and numbered. Veterinarians were identified with a ‘V’. Any words deemed necessary for clarification have been added to reported quotes in bold square brackets.

Interviews were coded initially based on the interview questions. From the initial data-derived coding [Braun and Clarke, 2013 ], a deeper iterative examination was undertaken of interviewees’ reflection around Hendra virus, including: How were the horse owners and veterinarians making sense of their experiences and why? What assumptions may underpin these considerations? The coding manual was developed by the first and third author, based on a latent coding approach [Braun and Clarke, 2013 ], with the themes being developed by all three authors. The final manual is found in Table 2 .


Table 2 : Interview coding manual.
PIC

The principles of inductive thematic analysis [Braun and Clarke, 2013 ], and interaction between data and existing research around expertise, uncertainty, risk and trust [Cairns, de Andrade and MacDonald, 2013 ; H. Collins and Evans, 2007 ; Douglas, 1986 ; Dunwoody, 1993 ; Engdahl and Lidskog, 2014 ], informed the development of the main themes [Davis, Stephenson, Lohm et al., 2015 ; Flyvbjerg, 2001 ] reported in this paper. Development of the themes was also informed by previous research into risk perception and uptake of suggested risk mitigation strategies for Hendra virus [Kung et al., 2013 ; Manyweathers, Field, Jordan et al., 2017 ; Manyweathers, Field, Longnecker et al., 2017 ; Taylor, Dhand et al., 2016 ].

3 Results

Communication between veterinarians and horse owners around the issue of Hendra virus and the release of the vaccine for horses has been characterised by dissent and complexity. This quote provides a snapshot of the communication challenges for all participants in the discourse.

One guy said to me, “How many people have died?” And I said, “Four people have died.” “How many horses?” And I couldn’t remember how many, so I said, “A hundred.” And he said, “Well, when 20 people have died and 2,000 horses have had Hendra, I will vaccinate my horses.”… I said, “So you’re happy if your 13-year-old daughter is one of the 16 more people who’ve got to die before you vaccinate your horses?” And I didn’t vaccinate his horse…The next day, he rang and said, “I don’t want that vet back on my property.” (V10)

Two main but interwoven themes around expertise were identified from the interview data obtained in this study — firstly, the management of uncertainty and its effects on trust, and secondly, how perception of expertise influenced inclusion in discussions.

3.1 Trust in times of uncertainty

The management of uncertainty and the impact this has on the development of trust between participants was identified as a significant contributor to the divisive nature of the communication among veterinarians and horse owners in this study. Management of the restricted permit for use of the vaccine played a particular role in undermining trust, as highlighted in this comment by a horse owner.

So in the first two years, they [vaccine manufacturer] changed their permit fractionally five times…But there’s no data added saying “We’ve tested now the 100,000 horses…and we’ve blood tested them, and we know that horses who have had a shot every six months have a titre of over whatever.” (O09)

The desire for evidence to support vaccine safety and efficacy was reflected in many of the interviews (n=6). Two respondents also commented about how changes were made to the permit without supporting information and how they considered this process very unsettling (n=2). This uncertainty contributed to the decision of two respondents who had started vaccinating their horses to discontinue the vaccine protocol. These comments included calls for transparency of data, and frustration with how requests for more information were dealt with. This perceived lack of transparency appears to have added to the general feeling that secrets were being kept and that horse and human welfare were not a priority.

The following comment from a veterinarian highlights the importance of transparency about what facts are known and what is still unknown.

This has been touted as much safer than what it actually is. I think people need to be given all the details. They need to know about Freedom of Information Act. We need to know what’s really going on and then it should be up to the individual. (V04)

A common theme was the desire for openness around what information is available to those who need to make decisions around the risk event (n=5). The ongoing silence or discordant interactions with pharmaceutical company representatives and veterinarians appeared to validate participants’ increasing fear around the vaccine and added to the sense that there was a conspiracy between veterinarians, the veterinary professional body, the pharmaceutical company and the vaccine registration body (AVPMA).

The type of interactions between horse owners and veterinarians were generally reported as being negative, but there were examples of positive participatory conversations. The following comment illustrates how trust can co-exist with uncertainty.

The anti-vaccination people say, “Well, there’s no guarantee that your horse is protected if it’s vaccinated.” But having spoken to my vets, I’m fairly comfortable with the fact that my horses are highly unlikely to contract Hendra virus after vaccination, and I have a bit more faith in them than I do the Facebook page . (O04)

In this situation, the negativity surrounding the uncertainty of the vaccine had been addressed openly within the veterinarian/horse owner relationship, resulting in some of the owner’s more at-risk animals being vaccinated. The language used by this horse owner allows for uncertainty around the extent of vaccine protection and a continuation of collaborative discourse.

When faced with people whose decisions differ to those supported by current scientific research, the common response by many of the veterinary participants was to assume that more information, or the right information would change people’s minds. This illustrates the prevalence of the deficit approach within science communication, that receivers of scientific information, in this case horse owners, are passive, empty vessels waiting to be filled, and that uncertainty needs to be eliminated.

I think we just need to get as much data as we can out there regarding safety and efficacies. You can try and convince people and that may not even be enough. (V06)

This approach to uncertainty and communication seemed to create a larger gap in the conversation and contributed to another issue around science communication and expertise — the need for a more participatory, two-way approach to science communication.

3.2 Consideration of local knowledge

The second theme to emerge in interviews involves how people and their expertise might be accepted into or rejected from the discourse, based on how their knowledge and experience was valued. The following comment from a veterinarian highlights the potential for exclusion of people who have differing opinions.

I don’t understand why people won’t vaccinate their horse. Why would you not? It’s almost a perfect vaccine…Causes some slight reactions, but has been shown to be 100 percent protective with very minimal side effects, bit of a sore neck for once in a while, if you need to have… a dose of bute [anti-inflammatory for horses] . There’s those that refuse to have it done that are probably the same tree-hugging hippies from Byron that refuse to have their kids vaccinated, and their kids are dying of meningococcal. But decisions are being made on political, financial, and public relations matters, not sound medical basis or sound medical decisions. It’s ridiculous. (V07)

At least two veterinary respondents appeared to disqualify the possibility of anything going wrong with the administration of the vaccine, and discussed the vaccine as though it was infallible. Yet regarding any vaccine as “almost perfect” is potentially blinding to the possibility of side effects and misses opportunities for further critical thinking and participatory discussion about vaccine efficacy.

The following comment offers an alternative approach to communication.

I just think that they came in and told us the best thing was for everybody [to vaccinate] without consultation. It was like all of a sudden the vaccination is here and you must do it and I think that was the driving force for a number of people to really take offence. And it really is a sense at being told that this is what they have to do regardless of their own management with their horses. I think that people would accept it a lot better if they weren’t forced. I just think that possibly people just need it to be done in a more softly, softly approach. And not being treated like idiots. I think we were treated like idiots to begin with. (O14)

This comment illustrates the consequences of a lack of inclusion of horse owners’ opinions, knowledge and expertise in the communication around the management of their animals, the vaccine launch and uptake of the Hendra vaccine for horses. The fact that many horse events were making vaccination a prerequisite for competition also contributed to a sense that some horses owners felt they were being forced to vaccinate. The majority of participants in this study who had vaccinated some of their animals (n=7) had done so in order to compete.

The following comment is from a horse owner who found herself on the receiving end of disbelief around her horse’s vaccine injury experience.

If you tell the average person you’ve got a problem with the vaccine, they look at you like you’ve just said “I’ve got syphilis.” Or “I’m still breastfeeding my teenager.” (O09)

Many horse owners spoke about their own experiences of vaccine side effects, or of those they knew about, with seven comments reflecting this. The perception was that the official communication arena excluded the possibility of side effects, and this contributed to the sense by horse owners of being ignored. This appeared to further undermine some owners’ trust in their veterinarian, the pharmaceutical company and the vaccine regulatory body. Some respondents reported feeling initially shocked by their experience of the vaccine side effects, then ostracised by their veterinarian when their concerns were raised, and then afraid to vaccinate again, or to vaccinate new horses. All this appeared to contribute to the perception that there was a conspiracy to protect the vaccine and its associated profits. The sense for some participants was that there was no one who would listen to or believe them.

The theme of inclusivity also reflects on who has the power or legitimacy to ask questions and interrogate scientific information. The following comment highlights the approach one horse owner took to consider the number of boosters required.

It’s not the vaccine I’m querying. It’s the amount of boosters we have to give…I would recommend everybody in the risk area vaccinate their horses as per permit. I’ve never been somebody who’s not trusting of authority. I’ve never been anyone to question that we should vaccinate…If you’d said to me “I don’t like vaccinations.” I would have said. “Oh, one of those crazy people.” (O09)

That comment reflects the exclusion some horse owners perceived when they did question aspects of the vaccine and protocol, even when those questions appeared to be scientifically robust and appropriate in the field of vaccine science.

Indeed, scientific thinking underpinned some of questions horse owners had about Hendra virus generally, as illustrated by this quote.

One thing I would like to find out…all the horses that contracted Hendra, I’d like to know what sort of condition they were in, how their immune system was, because most of them seem to be old or pregnant brood mares. I’d like to know all these sort of things and I think it should be studied to find out if the immune systems are down on most of the horses that get it naturally. (O04)

Many of the horse owners interviewed gave thoughtful and considered responses that included scientific consideration to questions around the virus and the vaccine. Multiple interviewees (n=6) indicated an interest in exploring the option of antibody testing to further the scientific knowledge base of protection duration offered by the vaccine. Four respondents were interested in the questions regarding infection dynamics: one horse infected in a paddock of many horses, for example. Indeed, interviewees generally had positive things to say about the vaccine. What they appeared concerned about was a perceived lack of inclusivity — that their opinions and knowledge were not valued, and that their questions were not adequately dealt with. If considered appropriately, these comments could contribute to more inclusive and participatory relationships and stakeholder-inclusive research.

An example of when inclusion of local knowledge and experience in communication strategies may have acted as a conduit to participatory communication and trust, but resulted in failure is highlighted in the next comment.

I thought…the vaccine’s all right. But then one of the fellas [pharmaceutical representatives] …spoke up and said, “Well as far as we know, bats don’t defecate while they’re flying.” And everyone around here has had their cars splattered by bats. It was just off putting to have somebody make a silly statement like that when they were supposed to be convincing us that what we’re doing is correct. And that set me back a little bit. (O12)

That comment illustrates communication impeded by perception of the vaccine manufacturer’s lack of awareness of local knowledge and bat physiology. It is not difficult to see how this interaction would seriously damage any existing trust in the pharmaceutical company and thus their ability to produce a product that can also be trusted. Many other respondents reported feeling that their knowledge around horse management and bat interactions were disregarded in the conversation. Interview data reflected how this lack of local context undermined the legitimacy with which the information and data from veterinarians and pharmaceutical representatives were received by horse owners in the study.

The veterinary profession as a whole, together with the national professional organisation, the Australian Veterinary Association (AVA), were also mentioned for their perceived lack of reflexive practice around how the vaccine was used and promoted (n=5). Some comments highlighted the damage this approach did to the veterinary profession generally and that it contributed to the divisive nature of communication around the vaccine.

I think the veterinary profession is doing a lot of damage to itself. When you get the president of the AVA touting online about special offers… the dental, Hendra/tetanus [vaccine for horses] package. Tetanus and Hendra and strangles [vaccine for horses] at the same time as having a sedation in dental. It’s not supposed to be like that. They’re not reading the interactions. They’re not reading the recommendations. I mean they’ve made themselves look like idiots. (V04)

Examples of where the vaccine permit guidelines were openly ignored were highlighted by interviewees, contributing to the underlying concern that the drive to vaccinate horses was driven less by welfare and horse/human health issues and more by profit or other interests.

The themes of uncertainty management and consideration of local knowledge were developed from the interview data. Their contribution to the divisive communication around Hendra virus and the vaccine for horse are discussed here.

4 Discussion

The value of open and frank communication around potentially controversial scientific issues cannot be underestimated [Davis, Stephenson, Lohm et al., 2015 ; Leiss and Powell, 2004 ; Manyweathers, Field, Longnecker et al., 2017 ]. However, for such communication to be possible, an arena where multiple voices can be heard is needed [Wynne, 1998 ], and the role of uncertainty understood [Brashers, 2001 ]. An issue which may appear clear and resolved to some stakeholders, may still be controversial and uncertain for others. Awareness of this possibility would provide scope for differing viewpoints within discussions [Han, Klein and Arora, 2011 ; Wynne, 1991 ].

The assumption that data arising from scientific research that informs communication strategies is unbiased and neutral is implicit within scientific training [H. M. Collins and Evans, 2002 ; Mogendorff, te Molder, van Woerkum et al., 2016 ; Wynne, 2006 ]. Wynne [ 1993 ] has suggested that self-awareness or reflexivity of how information is generated and used by those with expertise in risk decision making is vital. This should extend to management of uncertainty [Brashers, 2001 ]. Without such reflexivity, experts may consider their conclusions about risk are neutral, unbiased and free from any social and cultural context, with serious negative consequences for participatory communication [Metcalfe, 2019 ; Turner, 2001 ]. Scientific research is still firmly embedded in a positivist approach to knowledge [Seethaler et al., 2019 ], despite decades of research demonstrating the value of other ways of knowing [Douglas, 1986 ; Freudenburg, 1988 ; Slovic, 1999 ; Wynne, 1989 ]. The continuing default of using the deficit model of science communication corroborates this [Mogendorff, te Molder, Gremmen et al., 2012 ; Raps, 2016 ; Seethaler et al., 2019 ]. Appropriate consideration needs to be given to increasing the transparency of assumptions about neutrality in scientific research and the impact of those assumptions on risk communication.

The opaque nature of the scientific process also contributes to the apparent clash of paradigms around the development and adoption of new technology, including novel vaccines [Seethaler et al., 2019 ]. Clinical trials are not able to cover every biological, environmental, social and epidemiological variable and no vaccine is 100% protective nor risk free. A positivist approach to vaccine science is risky because it hinders discussion beyond 100% efficacy and uptake. Increased transparency of the process by which ideas are made concrete and how uncertainty is managed within the scientific framework can facilitate discussion around what is known, not yet known or not able to be known [Frewer et al., 2002 ; Keohane, Lane and Oppenheimer, 2014 ; Pearce et al., 2015 ].

Assuming a one size fits all, top down, binary approach to mitigating risk around disease outbreaks, compliant or noncompliant, does not promote inclusive communication or establishment of trusting relationships [Davis, Stephenson and Flowers, 2011 ]. Allowances should be made for uncertainty and alternate voices so that approaches to managing infectious diseases, based on unfolding but possibly obscured research will not continue to be plagued with fears of conspiracy and cover-up [Seethaler et al., 2019 ]. Without this, experts will continue to be ill-prepared for their role in the discourse.

The issues of uncertainty management and inclusivity highlighted in this study underpin greater institutional challenges around how science (in this case specifically veterinary science) is taught and evidence-based science communication practices implemented. Veterinary training in Australia excels in clinical practice, including diagnostics and surgery. What is lacking in their science training is capacity for deeper reflection of the impacts of source of expertise and of uncertainty when providing information that may facilitate or impede communication around decision making. An understanding of the complexity of the human animal bond, and the psychological impacts of disease management on animals owners, regardless of whether the animal is for production (profit) or pleasure is also important [Dashper, 2014 ; Mort et al., 2008 ]. If scientific training for veterinarians and others taught these concepts explicitly, participatory communication around risk communication may be facilitated. Future work in this area should be directed towards the development of evidence-based changes to science education aimed at improving the capacity of scientists, including veterinarians, to engage in progressive and participatory communication, especially around traditionally divisive topics.

This study was undertaken to examine perspectives of veterinarians and horse owners around Hendra virus and the release and uptake of the vaccine for horses. One aim was to inform improvements in future communication around emerging infectious zoonotic diseases. We recommend inclusion of the concepts of the roles of expertise and uncertainty in communication to improve disease management strategies in veterinary education.

We found evidence that institutional experts’ disqualification of local knowledge hampered participatory communication. This disqualification contributed to dissent around the recommended disease management strategies and disengagement and alienation of some decision makers. Trusting and open relationships between veterinarians and their clients are important. The goal for any zoonotic risk communication approach or policy should include strategies that facilitate improved communication between veterinarians and their clients, avoiding a one-size-fits-all, compliance focussed approach. This goal should include approaches that enhance messenger credibility [Fiske and Dupree, 2014 ] and encourage communication within existing relationships with respect for and understanding of animal owner knowledge [Higgins et al., 2016 ]. This study has highlighted the importance of improving experts’ capacity in reflexive practice and the urgent need for explicit teaching of evidence-based principles of science communication.

Our findings support the contention that how expertise of different stakeholders is valued influences the extent to which communication promotes trusting relationships that facilitate risk mitigation. Management of uncertainty that surrounds scientific research needs to be factored into communication strategies around responses to emerging diseases. This is particularly true when research is new and decisions are being made while many questions remain unanswered.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the horse owners and veterinarians who gave their time and thoughts. Funding from the Commonwealth of Australia, the New South Wales and Queensland governments was received for this project which was part of the National Hendra Virus Research Program. The first author held a research scholarship from the University of Western Australia.

References

Anderson, K. (1997). ‘A walk on the wild side: a critical geography of domestication’. Progress in Human Geography 21 (4), pp. 463–485. https://doi.org/10.1191/030913297673999021 .

Australian Government (2016). Compendium of findings from the National Hendra Virus Research Program. Barton, ACT, Australia: Rural Industries Research and Development Corporation. URL: https://www.agrifutures.com.au/product/compendium-of-findings-from-the-national-hendra-virus-research-program/ .

Bangerter, A., Green, E. and Gilles, I. (2011). ‘Editorial introduction’. Public Understanding of Science 20 (4), pp. 442–445. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662511406467 .

Brashers, D. E. (2001). ‘Communication and uncertainty management’. Journal of Communication 51 (3), pp. 477–497. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2001.tb02892.x .

Brashers, D. E., Goldsmith, D. J. and Hsieh, E. (2002). ‘Information seeking and avoiding in health contexts’. Human Communication Research 28 (2), pp. 258–271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00807.x .

Braun, V. and Clarke, V. (2013). Successful qualitative research: a practical guide for beginners. London, U.K.: SAGE Publications Ltd.

Brennan, M. L. and Christley, R. M. (2013). ‘Cattle producers’ perceptions of biosecurity’. BMC Veterinary Research 9 (1), p. 71. https://doi.org/10.1186/1746-6148-9-71 .

Brewer, P. R. and Ley, B. L. (2013). ‘Whose Science Do You Believe? Explaining Trust in Sources of Scientific Information About the Environment’. Science Communication 35 (1), pp. 115–137. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547012441691 .

Brinkmann, S. and Kvale, S. (2015). InterViews: learning the craft of qualitative research interviewing. 3rd ed. Los Angeles, CA, U.S.A.: SAGE Publications.

Broder, C. C., Weir, D. L. and Reid, P. A. (2016). ‘Hendra virus and Nipah virus animal vaccines’. Vaccine 34 (30), pp. 3525–3534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2016.03.075 .

Cairns, G., de Andrade, M. and MacDonald, L. (2013). ‘Reputation, relationships, risk communication and the role of trust in the prevention and control of communicable disease: a review’. Journal of Health Communication 18 (12), pp. 1550–1565. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2013.840696 .

Christley, R. M., Mort, M., Wynne, B., Wastling, J. M., Heathwaite, A. L., Pickup, R., Austin, Z. and Latham, S. M. (2013). ‘“Wrong, but useful”: negotiating uncertainty in infectious disease modelling’. PLoS ONE 8 (10), e76277. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076277 .

Collins, H. M. and Evans, R. (2002). ‘The Third Wave of Science Studies. Studies of Expertise and Experience’. Social Studies of Science 32 (2), pp. 235–296. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306312702032002003 .

Collins, H. and Evans, R. (2007). Rethinking expertise. Chicago, IL, U.S.A.: University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226113623.001.0001 .

Connor, M. and Siegrist, M. (2010). ‘Factors influencing people’s acceptance of gene technology: the role of knowledge, health expectations, naturalness and social trust’. Science Communication 32 (4), pp. 514–538. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547009358919 .

Convery, I., Bailey, C., Mort, M. and Baxter, J. (2005). ‘Death in the wrong place? Emotional geographies of the U.K. 2001 foot and mouth disease epidemic’. Journal of Rural Studies 21 (1), pp. 99–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrurstud.2004.10.003 .

Dashper, K. (2014). ‘Tools of the trade or part of the family? Horses in competitive equestrian sport’. Society & Animals 22 (4), pp. 352–371. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341343 .

Davis, M. D. M., Stephenson, N., Lohm, D., Waller, E. and Flowers, P. (2015). ‘Beyond resistance: social factors in the general public response to pandemic influenza’. BMC Public Health 15 (1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-1756-8 .

Davis, M., Stephenson, N. and Flowers, P. (2011). ‘Compliant, complacent or panicked? Investigating the problematisation of the Australian general public in pandemic influenza control’. Social Science & Medicine 72 (6), pp. 912–918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.016 .

Douglas, M. (1986). Risk acceptability according to the social sciences. London, U.K.: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Dowd, K., Taylor, M., Toribio, J.-A. L. M. L., Hooker, C. and Dhand, N. K. (2013). ‘Zoonotic disease risk perceptions and infection control practices of Australian veterinarians: call for change in work culture’. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 111 (1-2), pp. 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2013.04.002 .

Dunwoody, S. (1993). Reconstructing science for public consumption. Geelong, VIC, Australia: Deakin University.

Edson, D., Field, H., McMichael, L., Vidgen, M., Goldspink, L., Broos, A., Melville, D., Kristoffersen, J., de Jong, C., McLaughlin, A., Davis, R., Kung, N., Jordan, D., Kirkland, P. and Smith, C. (2015). ‘Routes of Hendra virus excretion in naturally-infected flying-foxes: implications for viral transmission and spillover risk’. PLOS ONE 10 (10), e0140670. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140670 .

Engdahl, E. and Lidskog, R. (2014). ‘Risk, communication and trust: Towards an emotional understanding of trust’. Public Understanding of Science 23 (6), pp. 703–717. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662512460953 .

Enticott, G. and Vanclay, F. (2011). ‘Scripts, animal health and biosecurity: the moral accountability of farmers’ talk about animal health risks’. Health, Risk & Society 13 (4), pp. 293–309. https://doi.org/10.1080/13698575.2011.575456 .

Field, H. E. (2016). ‘Hendra virus ecology and transmission’. Current Opinion in Virology 16, pp. 120–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coviro.2016.02.004 .

Field, H., Jordan, D., Edson, D., Morris, S., Melville, D., Parry-Jones, K., Broos, A., Divljan, A., McMichael, L., Davis, R., Kung, N., Kirkland, P. and Smith, C. (2015). ‘Spatiotemporal aspects of Hendra virus infection in pteropid bats (flying-foxes) in eastern Australia’. PLOS ONE 10 (12), e0144055. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144055 .

Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P. and Lichtenstein, S. (1982). ‘Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk’. The American Statistician 36 (3), p. 240. https://doi.org/10.2307/2683835 .

Fiske, S. T. and Dupree, C. (2014). ‘Gaining trust as well as respect in communicating to motivated audiences about science topics’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (Supplement 4), pp. 13593–13597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317505111 .

Flyvbjerg, B. (2001). Making social science matter: why social inquiry fails and how it can succeed again. New York, NY, U.S.A.: Cambridge University Press.

Franklin, S. (2001). ‘Sheepwatching’. Anthropology Today 17 (3), pp. 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8322.00058 .

Freudenburg, W. (1988). ‘Perceived risk, real risk: social science and the art of probabilistic risk assessment’. Science 242 (4875), pp. 44–49. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3175635 .

Frewer, L. J., Miles, S., Brennan, M., Kuznesof, S., Ness, M. and Ritson, C. (2002). ‘Public preferences for informed choice under conditions of risk uncertainty’. Public Understanding of Science 11 (4), pp. 363–372. https://doi.org/10.1088/0963-6625/11/4/304 .

Garvin, T. (2001). ‘Analytical Paradigms: The Epistemological Distances between Scientists, Policy Makers, and the Public’. Risk Analysis 21 (3), pp. 443–456. https://doi.org/10.1111/0272-4332.213124 .

Goldspink, L. K., Edson, D. W., Vidgen, M. E., Bingham, J., Field, H. E. and Smith, C. S. (2015). ‘Natural Hendra virus infection in flying-foxes — tissue tropism and risk factors’. PLOS ONE 10 (6), e0128835. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128835 .

Halpin, K., Young, P. L., Field, H. E. and Mackenzie, J. S. (2000). ‘Isolation of Hendra virus from pteropid bats: a natural reservoir of Hendra virus’. Journal of General Virology 81 (8), pp. 1927–1932. https://doi.org/10.1099/0022-1317-81-8-1927 .

Han, P. K. J. (2016). ‘The need for uncertainty: a case for prognostic silence’. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 59 (4), pp. 567–575. https://doi.org/10.1353/pbm.2016.0049 .

Han, P. K. J., Klein, W. M. P. and Arora, N. K. (2011). ‘Varieties of uncertainty in health care’. Medical Decision Making 31 (6), pp. 828–838. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989x10393976 .

Hernández-Jover, M., Higgins, V., Bryant, M., Rast, L. and McShane, C. (2016). ‘Biosecurity and the management of emergency animal disease among commercial beef producers in New South Wales and Queensland (Australia)’. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 134, pp. 92–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2016.10.005 .

Hernández-Jover, M., Taylor, M., Holyoake, P. and Dhand, N. (2012). ‘Pig producers’ perceptions of the influenza pandemic H1N1/09 outbreak and its effect on their biosecurity practices in Australia’. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 106 (3-4), pp. 284–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2012.03.008 .

Higgins, V., Bryant, M., Hernández-Jover, M., McShane, C. and Rast, L. (2016). ‘Harmonising devolved responsibility for biosecurity governance: the challenge of competing institutional logics’. Environment and Planning A: Economy and Space 48 (6), pp. 1133–1151. https://doi.org/10.1177/0308518x16633471 .

Hii, C., Dhand, N. K., Toribio, J.-A. L. M. L., Taylor, M. R., Wiethoelter, A., Schembri, N., Sawford, K., Kung, N., Moloney, B., Wright, T., Field, H. and Schemann, K. (2020). ‘Information delivery and the veterinarian-horse owner relationship in the context of Hendra virus in Australia’. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 179, p. 104988. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2020.104988 .

Holland, K., Blood, R. W., Imison, M., Chapman, S. and Fogarty, A. (2012). ‘Risk, expert uncertainty and Australian news media: public and private faces of expert opinion during the 2009 swine flu pandemic’. Journal of Risk Research 15 (6), pp. 657–671. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2011.652651 .

Holliday, A. (2002). Doing and writing qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA, U.S.A.: SAGE.

Horst, M. (2008). ‘In search of dialogue: staging science communication in consensus conferences’. In: Communicating science in social contexts. Ed. by D. Cheng, T. Gascoigne, B. Schiele, M. Claessens, J. Metcalfe and S. Shi. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer, pp. 259–274. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-8598-7_15 .

Jensen, K. K. (2004). ‘BSE in the U.K.: why the risk communication strategy failed’. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 17 (4–5), pp. 405–423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-004-5186-3 .

Keohane, R. O., Lane, M. and Oppenheimer, M. (2014). ‘The ethics of scientific communication under uncertainty’. Politics, Philosophy & Economics 13 (4), pp. 343–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470594x14538570 .

Kung, N., McLaughlin, A., Taylor, M., Moloney, B., Wright, T. and Field, H. (2013). ‘Hendra virus and horse owners — risk perception and management’. PLoS ONE 8 (11), e80897. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080897 .

Leiss, W. and Powell, D. (2004). Mad cows and mother’s milk. Montréal, QC, Canada: McGill-Queen’s University Press.

Longnecker, N. (2016). ‘An integrated model of science communication — More than providing evidence’. JCOM 15 (05), Y01. URL: https://jcom.sissa.it/archive/15/05/JCOM_1505_2016_Y01 .

Mahalingam, S., Herrero, L. J., Playford, E. G., Spann, K., Herring, B., Rolph, M. S., Middleton, D., McCall, B., Field, H. and Wang, L.-F. (2012). ‘Hendra virus: an emerging paramyxovirus in Australia’. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 12 (10), pp. 799–807. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1473-3099(12)70158-5 .

Manyweathers, J. (2017). ‘Straight from the horse’s mouth: risk perception and discourse of horse owners and veterinarians around Hendra virus’. Ph.D. thesis. Perth, WA, Australia: University of Western Australia. URL: https://research-repository.uwa.edu.au/en/publications/straight-from-the-horses-mouth-risk-perception-and-discourse-of-h .

Manyweathers, J., Field, H., Jordan, D., Longnecker, N., Agho, K., Smith, C. and Taylor, M. (2017). ‘Risk mitigation of emerging zoonoses: Hendra virus and non-vaccinating horse owners’. Transboundary and Emerging Diseases 64 (6), pp. 1898–1911. https://doi.org/10.1111/tbed.12588 .

Manyweathers, J., Field, H., Longnecker, N., Agho, K., Smith, C. and Taylor, M. (2017). ‘“Why won’t they just vaccinate?” Horse owner risk perception and uptake of the Hendra virus vaccine’. BMC Veterinary Research 13 (1), p. 103. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-017-1006-7 .

Mendez, D. H., Büttner, P., Kelly, J., Nowak, M. and Speare, R. (2016). ‘Difficulties experienced by veterinarians when communicating about emerging zoonotic risks with animal owners: the case of Hendra virus’. BMC Veterinary Research 13 (1), p. 56. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-017-0970-2 .

Mendez, D. H., Judd, J. and Speare, R. (2012). ‘Unexpected result of Hendra virus outbreaks for veterinarians, Queensland, Australia’. Emerging Infectious Diseases 18 (1), pp. 83–85. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1801.111006 .

Metcalfe, J. (2019). ‘Comparing science communication theory with practice: an assessment and critique using Australian data’. Public Understanding of Science 28 (4), pp. 382–400. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662518821022 .

Middleton, D., Pallister, J., Klein, R., Feng, Y.-R., Haining, J., Arkinstall, R., Frazer, L., Huang, J.-A., Edwards, N., Wareing, M., Elhay, M., Hashmi, Z., Bingham, J., Yamada, M., Johnson, D., White, J., Foord, A., Heine, H. G., Marsh, G. A., Broder, C. C. and Wang, L.-F. (2014). ‘Hendra virus vaccine, a one health approach to protecting horse, human and environmental health’. Emerging Infectious Diseases 20 (3), p. 379. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid2003.131159 .

Mogendorff, K., te Molder, H., Gremmen, B. and van Woerkum, C. (2012). ‘“Everyone may think whatever they like, but scientists…”: or how and to what end plant scientists manage the science-society relationship’. Science Communication 34 (6), pp. 727–751. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547011433887 .

Mogendorff, K., te Molder, H., van Woerkum, C. and Gremmen, B. (2016). ‘Turning experts into self-reflexive speakers’. Science Communication 38 (1), pp. 26–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547015615113 .

Mort, M., Convery, I., Baxter, J. and Bailey, C. (2008). ‘Animal disease and human trauma: the psychosocial implications of the 2001 U.K. foot and mouth disease disaster’. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 11 (2), pp. 133–148. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700801925984 .

Ochieng’, B. J. and Hobbs, J. E. (2016). ‘Incentives for cattle producers to adopt an E. Coli vaccine: an application of best-worst scaling’. Food Policy 59, pp. 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2015.12.004 .

Pearce, W., Brown, B., Nerlich, B. and Koteyko, N. (2015). ‘Communicating climate change: conduits, content and consensus’. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change 6 (6), pp. 613–626. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.366 .

Priest, S. H. (2006). ‘Public Discourse and Scientific Controversy A Spiral-of-Silence Analysis of Biotechnology Opinion in the United States’. Science Communication 28 (2), pp. 195–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547006293918 .

Queensland Government (2013). Hendra virus. Information for horse owners, handlers, competitors and event organisers . URL: https://www.daf.qld.gov.au/__data/assets/pdf_file/0005/57218/hendra-virus-info-pack-horse-owners.pdf .

— (2016). Hendra virus Equivac vaccine and its use by veterinary surgeons in Queensland . URL: https://www.parliament.qld.gov.au/work-of-committees/former-committees/AEC/inquiries/past-inquiries/rpt24-09-HendraVirusVacc .

Raps, B. G. (2016). ‘In science communication, why does the idea of a public deficit always return?’ Public Understanding of Science 25 (4), pp. 460–464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662516629748 .

Rogers, C. (2016). ‘The critical need for animal disaster response plans’. Journal of Business Continuity & Emergency Planning 9 (3), pp. 262–271. PMID: 26897622 .

Schemann, K., Firestone, S. M., Taylor, M., Toribio, J.-A. and Dhand, N. K. (2011). ‘Veterinarians as important biosecurity information providers during the 2007 equine influenza outbreak in New South Wales, Australia’. In: International One Health Congress (Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 14th–16th February 2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10393-010-0376-0 .

Seethaler, S., Evans, J. H., Gere, C. and Rajagopalan, R. M. (2019). ‘Science, values and science communication: competencies for pushing beyond the deficit model’. Science Communication 41 (3), pp. 378–388. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547019847484 .

Slovic, P. (1999). ‘Trust, emotion, sex, politics and science: surveying the risk-assessment battlefield’. Risk Analysis 19 (4), pp. 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1999.tb00439.x .

Taylor, M., Lynch, E., Burns, P. and Eustace, G. (2015). ‘The preparedness and evacuation behaviour of pet owners in emergencies and natural disasters’. Australian Journal of Emergency Management 30 (2), pp. 18–23. URL: http://search.informit.com.au/documentSummary;dn=176309304460819;res=IELHSS .

Taylor, M., Dhand, N., Toribio, J.-A., Wiethoelter, A., Schembri, N., Sawford, K., Kung, N., Field, H., Moloney, B. and Wright, T. (2016). Longitudinal cohort study of horse owners. HHALTER: horse owners and Hendra virus: a longitudinal study to evaluate risk. Final report. Barton, ACT, Australia: Rural Industries Research and Development Corporation. URL: http://era.daf.qld.gov.au/id/eprint/5066/ .

Triezenberg, H. A., Gore, M. L., Riley, S. J. and Lapinski, M. K. (2014). ‘Perceived risks from disease and management policies: an expansion and testing of a zoonotic disease risk perception model’. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 19 (2), pp. 123–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2014.844288 .

Turner, S. (2001). ‘What is the problem with experts?’ Social Studies of Science 31 (1), pp. 123–149. https://doi.org/10.1177/030631201031001007 .

Wiethoelter, A. K., Sawford, K., Schembri, N., Taylor, M. R., Dhand, N. K., Moloney, B., Wright, T., Kung, N., Field, H. E. and Toribio, J.-A. L. M. L. (2017). ‘“We’ve learned to live with it” — a qualitative study of Australian horse owners’ attitudes, perceptions and practices in response to Hendra virus’. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 140, pp. 67–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2017.03.003 .

Wynne, B. (1989). ‘Sheepfarming after Chernobyl: a case study in communicating scientific information’. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development 31 (2), pp. 10–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/00139157.1989.9928930 .

— (1991). ‘Knowledges in context’. Science, Technology, & Human Values 16 (1), pp. 111–121. https://doi.org/10.1177/016224399101600108 .

— (1993). ‘Public uptake of science: a case for institutional reflexivity’. Public Understanding of Science 2 (4), pp. 321–337. https://doi.org/10.1088/0963-6625/2/4/003 .

— (1998). ‘May the Sheep Safely Graze? A Reflexive View of the Expert-Lay Knowledge Divide’. In: Risk, Environment and Modernity: Towards a New Ecology. Chapter 2. Ed. by S. Lash, B. Szerszynski and B. Wynne. London, U.K.: SAGE Publications Ltd, pp. 44–83. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446221983.n3 .

— (2006). ‘Public Engagement as a Means of Restoring Public Trust in Science — Hitting the Notes, but Missing the Music?’ Community Genetics 9 (3), pp. 211–220. https://doi.org/10.1159/000092659 .

Yearley, S. (2000). ‘Making systematic sense of public discontents with expert knowledge: two analytical approaches and a case study’. Public Understanding of Science 9 (2), pp. 105–122. https://doi.org/10.1088/0963-6625/9/2/302 .

Authors

Jennifer Manyweathers. Corresponding author. Jennifer is a researcher and practicing veterinarian with a degree and teaching experience in science communication. Jennifer completed a PhD in risk perception of and communication among horse owners and veterinarians around Hendra virus and the vaccine for horses. Jennifer is interested in the role that social and psychological factors play in how decisions are made by all stakeholders in the biosecurity, surveillance and animal health arena. Contact phone: +61 4 1994 1552. E-mail: jmanyweathers@csu.edu.au .

Melanie Taylor. Mel is an Associate Professor in Organisational Psychology at Macquarie University, Australia. Prior to this, Mel was a member of the University of Western Sydney Disaster Response and Resilience Research Group. She joined UWS as a Senior Research Fellow in February 2007, following a 20 year career in Human Factors in the U.K. Mel’s current interests centre on emergency and disaster preparedness and protective responses, for example uptake of vaccination, compliance with public health and animal health recommendations, and animal health and biosecurity practices. Contact phone: +61 2 9850 8105. E-mail: mel.taylor@mq.edu.au .

Nancy Longnecker. Nancy is a Professor of Science Communication at the University of Otago, New Zealand. She has experience as an agricultural research scientist, professional science communicator and science communication academic. Nancy has worked with a broad range of people, including those within primary industries, indigenous peoples, museum visitors and school students. She and her research group examine factors that affect peoples’ attitudes towards science-related issues, impact of science outreach and how information influences attitudes and behaviour. Care is taken to respect different values and sources of knowledge. Contact phone: +64 3 4797885. E-mail: nancy.longnecker@otago.ac.nz .

Supplementary material

Available at https://doi.org/10.22323/2.19040201